Associations Between Academic Burnout And Social-Cognitive Factors: Does General Cognitive Ability Matter?
Abstract
Self-regulated learning theories emphasise the interaction of cognitive, motivational, behavioural, social and emotional factors. The current study examined how general cognitive ability moderates the association between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors, such as academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The sample of 289 Estonian 8th-grade students completed the questionnaires assessing academic burnout, academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies; 12 tasks on Raven’s matrices’ D-set were administered to measure students’ general cognitive ability. While maladaptive emotion regulation strategies explained more of the variance in academic burnout than adaptive strategies, none of these had a significant interaction effect with general cognitive ability. On the contrary, a unique pattern emerged regarding general cognitive ability as a moderator of the association between adaptive social-cognitive factors and academic burnout. More specifically, students with higher general cognitive ability, but lower academic buoyancy and/or who use less frequently adaptive strategies for emotion regulation, such as positive refocusing, positive reappraisal and putting into perspective, are more prone to academic burnout than the other student groups. Therefore, the study indicates that higher cognitive abilities can either be a blessing or a curse, depending on whether the student tends to apply or not to apply adaptive coping strategies. The findings support the idea of explicitly teaching proactive practices to prevent academic burnout in schools. Beyond that, the results support a deeper investigation of the role that cognitive abilities play in the relationship between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors (including behavioural emotion regulation strategies).
Keywords: Academic burnoutcognitive abilitiesmoderation
Introduction
Academic burnout is shown to be a serious problem among adolescents (for a review see Walburg, 2014) that should be explicitly addressed. Previous research has suggested that academic buoyancy, as well as cognitive emotion regulation strategies, may play an important role in preventing and/or mitigating academic burnout and proven that adolescents with better coping abilities have reported fewer burnout symptoms than their peers with worse ones (Vinter, (under review); Vinter, Aus, & Arro, under review, 2019). Self-regulated learning theories expand the use of more domain-specific concepts of students’ learning and well-being, like cognitive, metacognitive, motivational, behavioural, social and emotional components, by integrating these into the same conceptual framework (Panadero, 2017). Therefore, the constructs belonging rather to one or the other self-regulation domain, are still considered to interact strongly as it has been already shown in previous studies (Stevens, Holmberg, Lovejoy, & Pittman, 2014). Therefore, we may ask if already known associations between well-being constructs, such as academic burnout, and social-cognitive factors, such as academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies, can have different significance in student groups with different cognitive abilities. In order to pay more specific attention to student groups that may need different support for preventing or mitigating academic burnout, more research is needed in this domain.
The concepts of academic burnout, general cognitive ability, academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation
Associations between academic burnout and general cognitive ability, academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies
Associations between academic burnout and general cognitive ability have not been researched a lot but it has been shown that academic burnout is related to lower level of academic achievement (May, Bauer, & Fincham, 2015; Salmela-Aro et al., 2008; Salmela-Aro, Read, Minkkinen, Kinnunen, & Rimpela, 2018; Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro, 2014) and diminished attention and problem solving skills (May et al., 2015). However, the associations between academic burnout and academic achievement (as well as general cognitive ability) are not consistent in every study. Many studies have found a negative significant association between Grade Point Average (GPA) and academic burnout (May et al., 2015; Salmela-Aro et al., 2018). On the contrary, the study conducted among Estonian middle school students (Vinter et al., 2019) reported no significant direct associations between academic burnout and math skills as well as general cognitive ability. In another study (Kikas, Jõgi, Palu, Mädamürk, & Luptova, 2016) it was found that students who reported themselves being more indifferent achieved lower results in the math exam. However, at the same time, 9% of high achievers in math also reported themselves being rather indifferent (Kikas, Jõgi, et al., 2016).
Significant associations between burnout and buoyancy have been well documented. The research has shown moderate or strong negative correlations between these constructs, especially among nursing students (Garcia-Izquierdo, Rios-Risquez, Carrillo-Garcia, & de los Angeles Sabuco-Tebar, 2018; Ríos-Risquez, Sabuco-Tebar, Carrillo-Garcia, García-Izquierdo, & Solano-Ruiz, 2018), and also among adolescents some research has been done (Anyan & Hjemdal, 2016; Vinter et al., 2019; Ying, Wang, Lin, & Chen, 2016).
While frequent use of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies has been associated with higher level of buoyancy (Mestre, Nunez-Lozano, Gomez-Molinero, Zayas, & Guil, 2017; Min, Yu, Lee, & Chae, 2013), using maladaptive strategies, on the contrary, has been shown to be related to higher levels of depressive symptoms (Garnefski & Kraaij, 2014; Garnefski et al., 2001, 2002; Martin & Dahlen, 2005; Zlomke & Hahn, 2010), which, in turn, have been closely related to academic burnout (Salmela-Aro, Savolainen, et al., 2009). Although research on emotion regulation strategies in the academic context, is quite recent (Burić, Sorić, & Penezić, 2016), there are some studies that have shown the association specifically between academic burnout and emotion regulation (Seibert, Bauer, May, & Fincham, 2017; Vinter, under review; Vinter et al., under review).
Interaction effects of general cognitive ability and academic buoyancy and/or cognitive emotion regulation strategies explaining the level of academic burnout
In addition to direct associations between academic burnout and cognitive abilities, academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies, previous research has also found moderation effects regarding these constructs. For example, buoyancy has been found to moderate between emotional states and psychological health (Garcia-Izquierdo et al., 2018; Anyan & Hjemdal, 2016;Ying et al., 2016) as well as moderation effects of cognitive emotion regulation strategies have been documented. For example, moderation effects of several adaptive as well as maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies between bully victimization and emotional problems, such as depressive symptoms and anxiety, have been found among adolescents (see Garnefski & Kraaij, 2014).
All mentioned research has mainly focused on testing the moderation effects of conceptually related constructs. Still, there is some evidence of how conceptually not directly related constructs interact, such as cognitive, motivational and social-emotional constructs. For example, some person-oriented research has been done regarding this topic among Finnish-speaking ninth-grade students, where low emotional well-being was not found to be related to poor math and reading performance directly, but only together with low school motivation (Parhiala et al., 2018). Among university students, formal operational thought and verbal abstract reasoning were found to moderate the relation between depressive symptoms and actual vs ideal discrepancies (Stevens et al., 2014). Vinter and colleagues (2019) have also reported the association between academic burnout and buoyancy being significantly different in Estonian middle school student groups with different math skills; however, the same association was not found to exist in student groups with different general cognitive ability in the same study. The present study aims to broaden the scope of research mentioned above and test the same association in student groups with different general cognitive ability as measured by Raven progressive matrices (Raven, 1981). Moreover, the associations between academic burnout and cognitive emotion regulation strategies are also tested the same way.
Problem Statement
Although the literature on self-regulated learning theories suggests cognitive, motivational, social and emotional factors to interact, there are still mixed results in this topic. For example, some studies have shown that cognitive factors predict burnout directly (May et al., 2015), but other studies have not found direct, but indirect association, predicting academic burnout through interaction between math skills and academic buoyancy (Vinter et al., 2019). However, there is a lack of research concentrating on moderated studies in this field and especially associations between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors among middle school students with different levels of cognitive abilities have not been widely explored.
It could be suggested that general cognitive ability might be a moderator between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors and therefore, explain the level of academic burnout among student groups with different general cognitive ability. As a further idea, in case these relationships will become more clear, it might be suggested that to achieve emotional well-being in academic settings, students with different general cognitive ability should be slightly differently addressed.
Research Questions
Resulting from discussion mentioned above, the following research question was raised:
How is academic burnout associated with academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in student groups with different general cognitive ability?
Purpose of the Study
The aim of the study is to examine whether and how general cognitive ability would moderate the associations between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors, such as academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. In other words, whether these associations are different in student groups with different cognitive ability. To our best knowledge, no previous studies have yet examined general cognitive ability as a moderator in the relationship between social-cognitive factors and academic burnout among middle school students.
Research Methods
Participants and procedure
The study was a part of a larger project “General competencies and their assessment 2011-2014” (Kikas, Aus, et al., 2016). The sample consisted of 289 eighth-grade middle school students (146 girls and 143 boys) from seven Estonian schools in Tallinn.
The school principals from Tallinn middle schools were contacted and letters of informed consent were also sent for the schools who agreed to participate in the study. Only the students, whose parents did not return the letters of refusal, participated in the study. The data were gathered in January 2016 by researchers. The confidentiality of students’ answers was assured. The students completed online questionnaires and tasks assessing different forms of skills and social-cognitive aspects. All the tasks and questionnaires were prepared in Estonian.
Measures
Data analysis strategy
For data analysis statistical package SPSS 25.0, as well as Process Macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2019), were used. To test for the moderation effect of general cognitive ability, a separate set of ten hierarchical regression analyses were conducted with each exploring moderation of the relationship between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors. The analyses were conducted in two steps. In the first step, one of the social-cognitive factors and the moderator were entered into the regression model. In the second step, also the interaction effect between the social-cognitive factors and the moderator were entered into the regression models and it was checked if the interaction effect, as well as the change in explained variance (
Findings
Descriptive analyses and relationships between measures
Descriptive statistics, including means, standard deviations, minimum and maximum values, correlation matrices and internal consistencies of all measured constructs are presented in Table
The bivariate correlation coefficients between all measures were moderate, and therefore, referring to the distinctiveness of the measures used in the study. Also, the internal consistencies were good for every measure (Cronbach
To answer the research question, at first, ten separate hierarchical regression analyses of each of the ten social-cognitive factors on academic burnout were performed, including general cognitive ability as a potential moderator. The results of the moderation analyses are presented in Table
The strongest interaction effect between social-cognitive factors and general cognitive ability on academic burnout was found between general cognitive ability and academic buoyancy (see Table
Summarising, the negative association between academic burnout and academic buoyancy as well as adaptive emotion regulation strategies (e.g., positive refocusing, positive reappraisal and putting into perspective) tends to get stronger as a person’s general cognitive ability increases. In other words, the present findings provide evidence of a moderation effect of general cognitive ability on the relationship between social-cognitive factors and academic burnout. For further exploring the extent and nature of how social-cognitive factors and academic burnout are related in groups with different general cognitive ability, separate analyses on the factors showing significant interactions with general cognitive ability were performed (see Table
The association between academic burnout and academic buoyancy was negative and significant among all students regardless having high, average or low general cognitive ability (see Table
Regarding the association between academic burnout and positive reappraisal, it was negative and significant among students who had either high or average general cognitive ability (see Table
In sum, the results revealed that only associations between adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and academic burnout were moderated by general cognitive ability. The strongest effects between academic burnout and academic buoyancy, positive reappraisal, positive refocusing and putting into perspective, were found among students with higher general cognitive ability. More specifically, students who have higher general cognitive ability, but low academic buoyancy and/or who use less frequently above-mentioned adaptive strategies for emotion regulation, are more prone to academic burnout (see Figures
Conclusion
As there is not very clear understanding of how cognitive, motivational, social and emotional factors and psychological well-being interact, this study aimed to examine whether and how general cognitive ability would moderate the associations between academic burnout and social-cognitive factors, such as academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies. In other words, whether these associations are different in student groups with different general cognitive ability.
At first, the results provided evidence that the most relevant and strongest factor in predicting academic burnout among all the student groups is academic buoyancy. This indicates that all students who have more problems with recovering from everyday school problems are more prone to academic burnout, despite the level of general cognitive ability. Previous research has also shown buoyancy acting as a protective factor against burnout (Anyan & Hjemdal, 2016; Garcia-Izquierdo et al., 2018; Vinter et al., 2019; Ying et al., 2016). However, the strongest was the effect among the students with higher general cognitive ability: they are less prone to burnout if they have higher level of academic buoyancy and more prone when having lower level of buoyancy. In this context, this finding replicates the study where the analogous pattern was found especially for the students with high math skills (Vinter et al., 2019).
Although none of the maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies had any significant interaction effects with general cognitive ability on predicting academic burnout, it is important to note that maladaptive strategies accounted for more variance of academic burnout than adaptive strategies. This finding is in line with previous studies which have also shown associations between low academic well-being and using maladaptive patterns of functioning in school ( Korhonen, Linnanmaki, & Aunio, 2014) and students who report more psychopathological symptoms, such as depressive symptoms and higher level of anxiety, report also using more frequently maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Garnefski et al., 2001, 2002; Legerstee, Garnefski, Verhulst, & Utens, 2011).
On the contrary, concerning adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies, namely positive reappraisal, positive refocusing and putting into perspective, a significant interaction effect emerged between general cognitive ability and these adaptive strategies for predicting academic burnout. More specifically, the students with higher general cognitive ability and a reduced tendency towards using adaptive emotion regulation strategies are more likely to suffer from academic burnout than the group with higher general cognitive ability who use more frequently adaptive strategies. Moreover, this student group is more prone to burnout than all the groups with lower general cognitive ability. From this viewpoint, academic buoyancy and adaptive emotion regulation strategies have an analogous interaction effect on students with higher general cognitive ability. Previous studies have also reported that successful students can be more prone to academic burnout while reporting more depressive symptoms and higher level of stress, and/or higher performance orientation (Salmela-Aro et al., 2018; Tuominen-Soini & Salmela-Aro, 2014; Tuominen-Soini, Salmela-Aro, & Niemivirta, 2008) or lower academic buoyancy (Vinter et al., 2019). This pattern refers to the possible problem quite many Estonian middle school students may have: although they have shown excellent results in PISA studies (OECD, 2016), they may be more prone to academic burnout under pressure to achieve academically, especially when they have low social-emotional skills.
The results point to the need for teaching academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in schools to help to prevent and/or mitigate academic burnout among students. In addition to paying more attention to academic buoyancy and using maladaptive strategies, also teaching adaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies is important, especially for students with higher general cognitive ability, as the results of the study show, especially this group of students can benefit more from this.
Intervention programs, targeted to improve students’ coping skills, have proven to be successful, but they also have been characterised by inevitable context-specificity and short-term effects (Dray et al., 2017; Yeager & Walton, 2011). Therefore, it may be important to combine and integrate the elements of supporting students’ coping skills into a larger curriculum framework (Garcia-Izquierdo et al., 2018; Ríos-Risquez et al., 2018) and paying explicitly more attention to teaching these across all subjects. However, as the results indicated that adaptive social-cognitive factors were associated with academic burnout especially among students with higher levels of general cognitive ability, but not among students with lower levels (except for academic buoyancy), it can be contemplated that using cognitively more advanced strategies is more common for students with higher cognitive abilities. It has been proposed before that students with lower levels of cognitive abilities may benefit more from behavioural strategies (Stevens et al., 2014). Therefore, not only cognitive emotion regulation strategies should be taken into account while teaching emotion regulation strategies, but emotion regulation topics might cover teaching cognitive as well as behavioural emotion regulation strategies.
The current study is not without limitations. First, the cross-sectional data have limited potential for making conclusions about causality and longitudinal studies are more useful in examining the causality of the measures. Second, mainly self-reported questionnaires were used (except for general cognitive ability) which are subject to different biases, such as response bias. Also, because of the small sample size, the results are not directly generalisable to different contexts, and more studies with larger sample sizes and in different educational contexts should be conducted to confirm or reject the patterns found in this study. The results also further support a deeper investigation of the role that cognitive abilities play in the relationship between social-cognitive factors (including behavioural emotion regulation strategies) and academic burnout.
In conclusion, the current study provides insights into how cognitive factors, such as general cognitive ability, and social-cognitive factors, such as academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies and psychological well-being (burnout) interact. The results of the study show that the associations between academic burnout and academic buoyancy and cognitive emotion regulation strategies can have a different impact on student groups with different cognitive abilities. The study indicates that higher cognitive abilities can either be a blessing or a curse depending on whether the student tends to apply or not to apply adaptive coping strategies. Teaching explicitly cognitive emotion regulation strategies may help to reduce students’ level of academic burnout. In addition, behavioural emotion regulation strategies should be investigated further and might also be integrated into the curriculum framework as students with lower levels of general cognitive ability may benefit more from them. Therefore, special attention needs to be paid to educational measures helping all students to acquire constructive and effective ways to reflect on and react to negative school-related events and situations.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research. Writing of the article was supported by the project „TU TEE - Tallinn University as a promoter of intelligent lifestyle“ (no 2014-2020.4.01.16-0033) under activity A5 in the Tallinn University Centre of Excellence in Educational Innovation. The author would like to thank Professor Eve Kikas (Tallinn University) and her research team, especially Kati Aus and Grete Arro, as well as the students who participated. The author is also very grateful to Paul Seitlinger for revising the manuscript.
References
- Anyan, F., & Hjemdal, O. (2016). Adolescent stress and symptoms of anxiety and depression: Resilience explains and differentiates the relationships. Journal of Affective Disorders, 203, 213–220.
- Burić, I., Sorić, I., & Penezić, Z. (2016). Emotion regulation in academic domain: Development and validation of the academic emotion regulation questionnaire (AERQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 96, 138–147.
- Dray, J., Bartlem, K., Bowman, J., Bailey, J., Campbell, E., Wolfenden, L., … Palazzi, K. (2017). Systematic Review of Universal Resilience-Focused Interventions Targeting Child and Adolescent Mental Health in the School Setting. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(10), 813–824.
- Garcia-Izquierdo, M., Rios-Risquez, M. I., Carrillo-Garcia, C., & de los Angeles Sabuco-Tebar, E. (2018). The moderating role of resilience in the relationship between academic burnout and the perception of psychological health in nursing students. Educational Psychology, 38(8), 1068–1079.
- Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2014). Bully victimization and emotional problems in adolescents: Moderation by specific cognitive coping strategies? Journal of Adolescence, 37(7), 1153–1160.
- Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(8), 1311–1327.
- Garnefski, N., Legerstee, J., Kraaij, V., van den Kommer, T., & Teerds, J. (2002). Cognitive coping strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A comparison between adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 25(6), 603–611.
- Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream Science on Intelligence: An Editorial with 52 Signatories, History, and Bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 13–23.
- Hayes, A. F. (2019). The PROCESS macro for SPSS and SAS. Retrieved from http://processmacro.org/index.html
- Kikas, E., Aus, K., Kaldoja, M.-L., Arro, G., Kivi, V., & Luptova, O. (2016). Õpetajate toetamine üldpädevuste arendamisel [Supporting teachers on developing general competencies]. In Artiklid üldpädevustest (pp. 89–97). Retrieved from https://oppekava.innove.ee/wp-content/uploads/sites/6/2017/03/Yldpadevuste_kontsept.pdf
- Kikas, E., Jõgi, A.-L., Palu, A., Mädamürk, K., & Luptova, O. (2016). Põhikooli matemaatika lõpueksami tausta uuringu tulemused [Results of the study of middle school students’ final exam in mathematics]. Retrieved from http://haridusinfo.innove.ee/UserFiles/%C3%9Cldharidus/Uuringud/Aruanne%209.klassi%20matemaatika%20%C3%B5pioskused.pdf
- Korhonen, J., Linnanmaki, K., & Aunio, P. (2014). Learning difficulties, academic well-being and educational dropout: A person-centred approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 31, 1–10.
- Legerstee, J. S., Garnefski, N., Verhulst, F. C., & Utens, E. M. W. J. (2011). Cognitive coping in anxiety-disordered adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 319–326.
- Martin, A. (2013). Academic buoyancy and academic resilience: Exploring “everyday” and “classic” resilience in the face of academic adversity. School Psychology International, 34(5), 488–500.
- Martin, A., & Marsh, H. (2006). Academic Resilience and Its Psychological and Educational Correlates: A Construct Validity Approach. Psychology in the Schools, 43(3), 267–281.
- Martin, A., & Marsh, H. (2008). Academic buoyancy: Towards an understanding of students’ everyday academic resilience. Journal of School Psychology, 46(1), 53–83.
- Martin, R., & Dahlen, E. (2005). Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1249–1260.
- May, R. W., Bauer, K. N., & Fincham, F. D. (2015). School burnout: Diminished academic and cognitive performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 42, 126–131.
- Mestre, J. M., Nunez-Lozano, J. M., Gomez-Molinero, R., Zayas, A., & Guil, R. (2017). Emotion Regulation Ability and Resilience in a Sample of Adolescents from a Suburban Area. Frontiers In Psychology, 8.
- Min, J.-A., Yu, J. J., Lee, C.-U., & Chae, J.-H. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies contributing to resilience in patients with depression and/or anxiety disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(8), 1190–1197.
- OECD. (2016). PISA 2015 Results (Volume I): Excellence and Equity in Education. Paris: OECD Publishing.
- Panadero, E. (2017). A Review of Self-regulated Learning: Six Models and Four Directions for Research. Frontiers In Psychology, 8.
- Parhiala, P., Torppa, M., Vasalampi, K., Eklund, K., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Aro, T. (2018). Profiles of school motivation and emotional well-being among adolescents: Associations with math and reading performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 196–204.
- Raven, J. (1981). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and mill hill vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.
- Ríos-Risquez, M. I., Sabuco-Tebar, E., Carrillo-Garcia, C., García-Izquierdo, M., & Solano-Ruiz, C. (2018). Connections between academic burnout, resilience, and psychological well-being in nursing students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 74(12), 2777–2784.
- Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., & Jokela, J. (2008). Does school matter? The role of school context in adolescents’ school-related burnout. European Psychologist, 13(1), 12–23.
- Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). School burnout inventory (SBI) reliability and validity. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 48–57.
- Salmela-Aro, K., Read, S., Minkkinen, J., Kinnunen, J. M., & Rimpela, A. (2018). Immigrant status, gender, and school burnout in Finnish lower secondary school students: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(2), 225–236.
- Salmela-Aro, K., Savolainen, H., & Holopainen, L. (2009). Depressive Symptoms and School Burnout During Adolescence: Evidence from Two Cross-lagged Longitudinal Studies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(10), 1316–1327.
- Seibert, G. S., Bauer, K. N., May, R. W., & Fincham, F. D. (2017). Emotion regulation and academic underperformance: The role of school burnout. Learning and Individual Differences, 60, 1–9.
- Stevens, E. N., Holmberg, N. J., Lovejoy, M. C., & Pittman, L. D. (2014). When do self-discrepancies predict negative emotions? Exploring formal operational thought and abstract reasoning skills as moderators. Cognition & Emotion, 28(4), 707–716.
- Tuominen-Soini, H., & Salmela-Aro, K. (2014). Schoolwork engagement and burnout among Finnish high school students and young adults: Profiles, progressions, and educational outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 649–662.
- Tuominen-Soini, H., Salmela-Aro, K., & Niemivirta, M. (2008). Achievement goal orientations and subjective well-being: A person-centred analysis. Learning and Instruction, 18(3), 251–266.
- Vinter, K. (under review). Academic Burnout Among Estonian Middle School Students: a Person-Oriented, Longitudinal Study.
- Vinter, K., Aus, K., & Arro, G. (under review). Adolescent Girls’ and Boys’ Academic Burnout and Its Associations with Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies.
- Vinter, K., Aus, K., & Arro, G. (2019). Akadeemiline läbipõlemine eesti ja vene õppekeelega õpilaste hulgas [Academic burnout among Estonian and Russian middle school students]. Estonian Journal of Education, 7(1), 127–155.
- Walburg, V. (2014). Burnout among high school students: A literature review. Children and Youth Services Review, 42, 28–33.
- Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-Psychological Interventions in Education: They’re Not Magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267–301.
- Ying, L., Wang, Y., Lin, C., & Chen, C. (2016). Trait resilience moderated the relationships between PTG and adolescent academic burnout in a post-disaster context. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 108–112.
- Zlomke, K. R., & Hahn, K. S. (2010). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies: Gender differences and associations to worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(4), 408–413.
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
07 November 2019
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-071-6
Publisher
Future Academy
Volume
72
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-794
Subjects
Psychology, educational psychology, counseling psychology
Cite this article as:
Vinter, K. (2019). Associations Between Academic Burnout And Social-Cognitive Factors: Does General Cognitive Ability Matter?. In P. Besedová, N. Heinrichová, & J. Ondráková (Eds.), ICEEPSY 2019: Education and Educational Psychology, vol 72. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 57-71). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2019.11.5