Cognitive Stimulation In Adolescents At Risk Of Exclusion Using The BAPNE® Method

Abstract

In this paper we study the viability of an intervention using cognitive stimulation through the BAPNE® Method to benefit a group of young adolescent in an educational establishment in an area of very difficult socio-economic conditions. In addition, experimental research has been also conducted to demonstrate that this intervention can improve impulse control amongst the group under study. The experiment was carried out with a total of N=70 subjects divided between an experimental group and a control group. The intervention in the experimental group was carried out following the series of activities in the BAPNE® Method set out in the Bodypercussion- Programación didáctica (Volumes 1 and 2) teaching manuals during 25 weeks but the control group followed their normal music curriculum. The evaluation tools used to measure executive function were: the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT - F.A.S. & Animals), the Digit Span subtest (Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward), the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) and the Trail Making Test (TMT). The data collection was analysed statistically using IBM SPSS v24 for Mac software and the results show that, between the pre- and the post-test, this intervention has visibly improved the inhibitory control and the verbal fluency amongst the experimental group.

Keywords: AdolescenceBAPNE® methodexecutive functionsmusicpsychomotor coordinationrisk of exclusion

Introduction

Various authors have studied the effects of music on child development and have shown that the practice of music improves social integration (Fabra-Brell & Romero-Naranjo, 2017; Kokotsaki & Hallam, 2007) and contributes notably to cognitive development (Altenmüller & Gruhn, 2002). It has been proven that musical training produce specific improvements in the executive functions (EF), including childhood singing games and games involving motor coordination (Sulkin & Brodsky, 2007). Above all, as signalled by recent studies, they particularly improve inhibitory control (Sachs, Kaplan, Sarkissian, & Habibi, 2017; Habibi, Damasio, Ilari, Elliott, & Damasio, 2018). For this reason, there are an increasing number of studies which show the link between practising music, motor development, coordination and improvement in the EF (Jaschke, Honing, & Scherder, 2018; Cameron et al, 2012; Piek et al., 2004) as well as in language skills in adolescence (Tierney, Krizman, & Kraus, 2015).

In this sense, and with reference to the group under study in this paper, it is necessary to highlight the significant degree of biological and psychosocial change during adolescence. A suitable social context is needed during this time so that an adolescent can become, by means of interaction with the constant changes in their social environment, a successful adult (Blakemore & Mills, 2014). Therefore, despite the fact that adolescence is a period which generally shows an overall improvement in cognition, when considering the socio-educational context of the group under study it is important to bear in mind that long-lasting exposure to a damaging environment of social exclusion may increase the risk of suffering from several different development disorders, such as mood disorders (Blakemore, 2008). The American Psychological Association (2003) sees social exclusion and poverty as two phenomena which affect individuals both physically and mentally, and which are linked to socioeconomic status (Saegert et al., 2007). Furthermore, it is known that the EF are crucial for carrying out both cognitive and motor learning processes (Diamond, 2000) and it has been shown that growing up in areas of low socioeconomic status (SES) increases the risk of delayed motor, language, and reading development (Kraus et al., 2014; McPhillips & Jordan-Black, 2007). But, on the other hand, studies point to the fact that the development of EF, particularly inhibitory control, is linked both to attention span and motor development (Pons-Terrés et al., 2014; Sergeant, 2000) and it has been shown that improved motor coordination development in children has a positive effect on processing of symbols, both in terms of language and mathematics (Berger, 2010; Holtzer et al., 2007).

In this way, despite the fact that its specific aim is not the teaching of music, the BAPNE® Method (Romero-Naranjo, Summer, & Palencia Pérez, 2012) has emerged as an innovative strategy of facilitated musical training which aims primarily to stimulate and promote the socioemotional and cognitive development (Cozzutti, Guaran, Blessano, & Romero-Naranjo, 2017; Jiménez-Molina, Vicedo-Cantó, Sayago-Martínez, & Romero-Naranjo, 2017). Furthermore, due to its multidimensional contribution (to cognitive, socioemotional, and the psychomotor development), the BAPNE® Method promotes development of the whole child, as it fosters other aspects which are essential for reaching adequate psycho-evolutionary development (Moral-Bofill, Romero Naranjo, Albiar-Aliaga, & Cid-Lamas, 2015; Conti & Romero-Naranjo, 2017; Cozzutti, Blessano, De Biaggio, Tomasin, & Romero-Naranjo, 2017; Fabra-Brell & Romero-Naranjo, 2017; Presti & Romero-Naranjo, 2017). Therefore, the BAPNE® Method (based on biomechanics, anatomy, psychology, neuroscience, and ethnomusicology, the disciplines which give rise to the acronym) is focussed on the benefits of neuromotor and multiple intelligences development (Romero-Naranjo et al., 2012; Everest & Gardner, 2016). The method is based on the use of the teaching of body percussion (Trives-Martínez et al., 2014) and the practice of music, without using instruments (the body is the instrument), as a neuromotor strategy based on the dissociation of the lower and upper limbs and the voice to stimulate cognitive, socioemotional, psychomotor and neuro-rehabilitative development. In this way, and through direct musical practice, linked to movement, rhythm, melody, motor coordination and language, the BAPNE® Method develops processes such as multi-tasking, various types of attention, working memory, inhibitory control (Jauset-Berrocal, Tripovic, & Romero-Naranjo, 2014), and uses stimuli which interact with other non-musical areas of the brain (Sulkin & Brodsky, 2007).

Thus, the structures and neuroanatomical functions of these non-musical areas are developed through the transfer effect (Jaschke et al., 2018; Rauscher & Hinton, 2006) drived by neurocognitive plasticity, which is exercised by the musical training (Herholz & Zatorre, 2012).

Problem Statement

The BAPNE® Method has been used as a means of therapy and treatment (Romero-Naranjo & Naranjo, 2013) for groups considered at risk. These have included a group of drug addicts in rehabiliation (Romero-Naranjo et al., 2014), individuals suffering from dementia (Cavan, Naranjo, & Bagolin, 2017), as a means of social inclusion, as a treatment for dyslexia (Marcuzzi & Romero-Naranjo, 2017) and amongst patients suffering from depression (Salerno, Cefaratti, & Romero-Naranjo, 2017). Given the diverse nature of the groups studied thus far, these studies suggest that the BAPNE® Method can be effective amongst populations considered to be at social risk. However, as far as we know, by looking over the review articles (Romero-Naranjo, 2013), academic metasearch engines (Web of Science, Jstor, PubMed, Eric, Scopus, CINHAL and PEDro) and consulting high-impact articles, this is the first experimental and viability study of the BAPNE® Method amongst a group of young teenagers at risk of social exclusion

Research Questions

  • Is it viable to put into place a programme of cognitive stimulation using the BAPNE® Method amongst a group of young teenagers at risk of social exclusion?

  • Can the BAPNE® Method significantly increase the development of the EF in the group under study and improve their social surroundings?

Purpose of the Study

  • To evaluate the levels of risk and vulnerability of a pilot group of young teenagers in their first year of secondary school in an educational establishment in an area of very difficult socio-economic conditions.

  • To check the viability of the use of a programme of cognitive stimulation using the BAPNE® Method amongst a group of young adolescents at risk of social exclusion.

  • To show through experiments that by means of a programme of cognitive stimulation using the BAPNE® Method amongst a group of young adolescents at risk of social exclusion it is possible to significantly increase the development of the EF.

Research Methods

Context

The experimental investigation was carried out with a total of N=70 subjects in their first year of secondary school, divided between an experimental group and a control group, both from areas with similar socio-economic and educational characteristics in the province of Barcelona.

Experimental group

The intervention involving cognitive stimulation by means of the BAPNE® Method was carried out at the INS Pedraforca, situated in the city of Hospitalet de Llobregat. This establishment has been classed by the regional Government of Catalunya’s Educational Inspection Board as being in an area of very difficult socio-economic conditions (D’Avaluació, 2017). Furthermore, 95% of the student body is of immigrant origin and 56% are recent immigrants. This is one of the reasons why the student body of these establishments is considered a group of social exclusion. The experimental group (n=39) consisted of 19 girls and 20 boys aged between 12 and 15 years of age.

Initial assessment

In order to check the viability of the study in terms of the choice of group and their being at risk of social exclusion, given the multiple risk factors to which the studied population is exposed (low SES, segregation, newly-arrived students, etc.), an initial assessment based on interviews and projective techniques as a empirical instruments of qualitative methodology consistent with the complexity of the population under study (Sneiderman, 2011) was carried out.

This was done by using the emotional and development indicators proposed by Koppitz (2006) for the Human Figure Drawings Test (HFDT) alongside storytelling and semi-structured interviews with teachers and school staff. The teaching and non-teaching staff from the establishment were selected according to their relationships with the students under study. They included the group's music teacher, two form tutors, the child-psychologist, the social integration coordinator and the school receptionist.

In this way, using the development and emotional upset indicators of this method, the results show that 31% of the student body is below the level of maturity expected for their age and 74% shows signs of psychological stress. Furthermore, the interviews with the academic staff reveal a high degree of segregation within the school, high levels of absenteeism, illiteracy, poor study habits, little family involvement in their child’s education, and traumatic experiences due to the break-up of the family unit caused by migration. Therefore, on the basis of these results, we consider that the group under study can be seen to be at risk of social exclusion.

Control group

The control group of the study was chosen from the IES Verge de les Neus, situated in the city of Santa Coloma (area of similar socio-economic characteristic to Hospitalet de Llobregat). The control group (n=31) consisted of 16 girls and 15 boys aged between 12 and 14 years of age.

Experimental evaluation tools and data collection procedure

In order to measure initial EF levels, a set of neuropsychological measurement tests was designed using the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) in its spanish adaptation published by TEA (2010), the Trail Making Test (TMT), the Controlled Oral Word Association Tests (COWAT; Benton, Hamsher, & Sivan, 1989) in its 'F.A.S.' and 'Animals' variants of the spanish adaptation for young population (Casals-Coll et al., 2013) and two Digit Span subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV: Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward). The pre-test was carried out on both groups during November and, following this, the experimental group undertook a training programme by means of the BAPNE® Method between November 2016 and May 2017, participating in sessions twice a week for 50 minutes at a time. Throughout the programme, 33 such sessions were carried out within music lessons with the experimental group. However, the control group followed their normal scheme of work for music. Once the programme was completed, a post-test was carried out on both groups between May and June 2017.

BAPNE ® Method

Following the protocol of activities set out in the BAPNE® Method, which are published and set out in sequence in various manuals and articles with the Glossary, especially in the volumes ‘Teaching programmes Volume 1 and 2’, the intervention carried out on the experimental group could be adapted to the characteristics of the chosen population.

Viability study

To study the feasibility of the intervention, the music teacher was encouraged not to intervene to see if the BAPNE® trainers themselves could manage responsibility for the group. However, the groups were violent towards each other and paid minimal attention to the trainers. It could clearly be seen that a subsection of the group were passive participants and another subsection were overly active. The group would blame any classmates who made mistakes. After four weeks, and some violent incidents later, it was decided that the teacher should take the lead again and be responsible for the group. The fact that the trainer stopped being the authority figure was a positive change. As the intervention went on, the group showed a greater level of interest and enthusiasm to participate. On occasion, a student would start the session visibly upset, but would end the session smiling and happily participating. When students were removed from the group due to bad behaviour, they tended to show reluctance to leave the session. During the sessions, a healthy atmosphere of controlled freedom and free expression came about. That is why at a later stage, it was suggested that a assessment at the end of the sessions to allow the group to express any worries, which resulted in a significant decrease in violence between companions.

Findings

To verify the validity of the results collected, a model to compare the mean averages of the experimental and the control groups was used. The t-Student test was employed, with a level of statistical significance of 0.05 (p<0.05). Furthermore, as there were two independent samples (control group n=31 and experimental group n=39), it was assumed that the total population N=70 showed a normal distribution. The statistical program used was IBM SPSS v24 for Mac.

Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT)

Using the number of error as an indicator of the inhibitory control, especially in the interference test (C part) the pre-test for the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) did not show significant differences between the two groups (the control group and the experimental group). However, the post-test showed statistically significant differences between the average number of errors made on the C part between the two groups, with a P value of 0.035 (Table 01). The difference between the averages in the experimental group and the control group was -0.865 (Table 1).

In terms of difference between the pre- and post-tests, when comparing the average number of errors made in part A, statistically significant differences were found between the two groups with a P value of 0.034. Similar results also occurred in C part with a P value of 0.02 (Table 01). This shows that the experimental group saw a greater reduction in the number of errors made than the control group (Figure 1 & 2).

Figure 1: Average number of errors in the post-test scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test is lower in the experimental group (-0'865) with a more than enough statistical significance (p=0.035).
Average number of errors in the post-test scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test is lower in the experimental group (-0'865) with a more than enough statistical significance (p=0.035).
See Full Size >
Figure 2: Difference between the post- and pre-test error scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test, which show a substantial reduction of errors in the experimental group (-88%) in comparison to the control group (-12%)
Difference between the post- and pre-test error scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test, which show a substantial reduction of errors in the experimental group (-88%) in comparison to the control group (-12%)
See Full Size >
Figure 3: Average number of errors in the post-test scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test
Average number of errors in the post-test scores in the Stroop Color and Word Test
See Full Size >

Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-FAS)

In order to study verbal fluency, we used COWAT with the phonetic test of the letters 'F', 'A' and 'S' as an indicator of verbal fluency. Statistically significant differences were recorded with a P value of 0.001 (see Table 2) in the difference in the average number of words between the pre- and post-tests in both groups. In terms of difference between the pre- and post-tests in the total number of errors, a greater difference was found amongst the experimental group (see Figure 3 ).

Figure 4: Average score for number of errors in post-test in the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-FAS) is a lower for the experimental group with a substantial statistical significance (p=0.001).
Average score for number of errors in post-test in the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-FAS) is a lower for the experimental group with a substantial statistical significance (p=0.001).
See Full Size >
Figure 5: Difference between the pre- and post-tests of the total number of errors in the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-FAS). In the experimental group, a greater decrease in the number of errors can be seen.
Difference between the pre- and post-tests of the total number of errors in the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT-FAS). In the experimental group, a greater decrease in the number of errors can be seen.
See Full Size >

Conclusion

This study confirms the viability of a programme of intervention using the BAPNE® Method with the aim of stimulating the cognitive development of adolescents at risk of social exclusion. This represents a step forward in the application of this method with special groups. Furthermore, by responding to the social demands of the group under study, whose socioeconomic characteristics have a negative impact upon their cognitive and behavioural development (Kraus et al., 2014; OECD, 2011; Walper, 2009; McPhillips & Jordan-Black, 2007; Saegert et al., 2007), the results of this study empirically support the thesis that the BAPNE® Method can be considered a valid and effective non-pharmacological strategy to notably improve inhibitory control and verbal fluency amongst children and adolescents in highly difficult socio-economic conditions.

It is important to remember that the age group most at risk of poverty and social exclusion in the Member States (MS) of the European Union (EU) are those aged under 18 (Statistics Explained, 2015; Ron, 2013). It is for this reason that, in order to reduce social expenditure in the future, the European Commission, alongside other EU and MS institutions, are currently cooperating by means of the Open Method of Coordination (OMC) to combat the consequences of social exclusion and those factors which perpetuate the intergenerational transmission of poverty (ITP) (Ron, 2013) using strategies included in the Lisbon European Council and the current Europe 2020 (European Council, 2000 and 2010). In this way, the current study supports the theoretical constructs which report about lack of involvement and interest in education of children, typically exhibited by parents in families with low SES (Younge, 2013), the consequences of segregation processes in the education system (Jahan, 2017) and how a low SES is directly linked to cognitive impulsivity in children (Arán-Filippetti & Richaud de Minzi, 2012; Younge, 2013). It has been shown that there is a directly proportional relationship between SES and the level of development of inhibitory control, one of the most important EF related to SES in order to appropriately direct thought (Sheehy-Skeffington & Rea, 2017). Thus, from the recently discoveries, and bearing in mind that inhibitory control is the ability which allows our attention not to be distracted and diverted towards unfavourable sources (Lavie, 2010), it can be seen that developing inhibitory control is a crucial process for children and adolescents to help them avoid disruptive behaviours, to study and to achieve at school, all of them factors that contribute to leave ITP (OECD, 2011; Duarte, Ferrando-Latorre, & Molina, 2018). It is also important to consider that, whilst the link between cognitive achievement and SES has been shown, the exact causes of this phenomenon are still unknown (Arán-Filippetti & Richaud de Minzi, 2012). Furthermore, although social exclusion is strongly linked to poverty (Littlewood, Glorieux, & Jönsson, 2017), it is important to remember that ITP also depends on non-economic factors (such as education levels, sociocultural levels and the physical and mental health of householders), which have been shown to be even more decisive factors than poverty itself for ITP (Vauhkonen, Kallio, Kauppinen, & Erola, 2017).

Therefore, in the light of the review and considering the EF as non-economic factors which influence ITP, from the results presented in this paper, we can propose the working hypothesis that by strengthening inhibitory control, which is crucial for the development of the processes of decision making associated to SES (Sheehy-Skeffington & Rea, 2017), planning educational intervention using the BAPNE® Method could be a cognitive stimulation strategy to help combat the sociocultural causes of ITP. This study only offers preliminary support to our thesis regarding the use of the BAPNE® Method amongst adolescents at risk of social exclusion. However, considering the responsibilities of public institutions, we can argue that the BAPNE® Method can be considered a suitable compensatory strategy in interventions with young adolescents in educational establishments in very difficult socio-economic conditions. And finally, we can also argue that, although this is an educational strategy (therefore we are talking about a solution in the medium- and long-term), a large-scale implementation could be an excellent opportunity to investigate and evaluate the effectiveness and impact that such action could have on society, beyond economic measures against the consequences of ITP.

Acknowledgments

We thank the head teachers, the teaching, and non-teaching staff in both establishments – IES Verge de les Neus and IES Pedraforca –, in particular we thank Estíbaliz Solaun Unzaga, music teacher of the experimental group, for her extraordinary support. And finally, we should also like to extend our special thanks to Diego Sánchez González from Xera Educational Association and Perfecto Jose Herrera Boyer from UPF's Music Technology Group for their invaluable collaboration.

References

  1. Altenmüller, E., & Gruhn, W. (2002). Brain mechanisms. In R. Parncutt & G.E. McPherson (Eds.), The science and psychology of music performance – Creative strategies for teaching and learning (pp. 63–82). New York: Oxford University Press.
  2. American Psychological Association. (2003). Resolution on poverty and socioeconomic status. Roeper Review, 25(3), 103-105.
  3. Arán-Filippetti, V., & Richaud de Minzi, M. C. (2012). A structural analysis of executive functions and socioeconomic status in school-age children: Cognitive factors as effect mediators. The Journal of genetic psychology, 173(4), 393-416.
  4. Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. D., & Sivan, A. (1989). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
  5. Berger, S. E. (2010). Locomotor expertise predicts infants’ perseverative errors. Developmental psychology, 46(2), 326.
  6. Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267.
  7. Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual review of psychology, 65, 187-207.
  8. Cameron, C. E., Brock, L. L., Murrah, W. M., Bell, L. H., Worzalla, S. L., Grissmer, D., & Morrison, F. J. (2012). Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child development, 83(4), 1229-1244.
  9. Casals-Coll, M., Sánchez-Benavides, G., Quintana, M., Manero, R. M., Rognoni, T., Calvo, L., & Pena-Casanova, J. (2013). Spanish normative studies in young adults (NEURONORMA young adults project): norms for verbal fluency tests. Neurología (English Edition), 28(1), 33-40.
  10. Cavan, V., Naranjo, J. R., & Bagolin, M. (2017). The Efficacy of BAPNE Method in Dementia Treatment: A Research Proposal in Friuli Venezia Giulia, Italy. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1223-1228.
  11. Conti, D., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Body Percussion and Voice, Time and Pitch: An Exercise in «Singing BAPNE®» Methodologically Analyzed. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 798-803.
  12. Cozzutti, G., Blessano, E., De Biaggio, C., Tomasin, B., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Body Solfege in the BAPNE Method – Measures and Divisions. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1572-1575.
  13. Cozzutti, G., Guaran, F., Blessano, E., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Effects on executive functions in the BAPNE Method; a study on 8-9 years old children in Friuli Venezia Giulia, Italy. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 900-907.
  14. D'Avaluació, C. S. (2017). L’avaluació de quart d’ESO 2013. Consell Superior d’Avaluació del Sistema Educatiu.
  15. Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child development, 71(1), 44-56.
  16. Duarte, R., Ferrando-Latorre, S., & Molina, J. A. (2018). How to escape poverty through education? Intergenerational evidence in Spain. Applied Economics Letters, 25(9), 624-627.
  17. Everest, S. F., & Gardner, H. (2016). Estructuras de la mente: la teoría de las inteligencias múltiples. Fondo de Cultura Económica/México.
  18. Fabra-Brell, E., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Social competence between equals through body percussion according to method BAPNE in secondary students. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 829-836.
  19. Habibi, A., Damasio, A., Ilari, B., Elliott, S.M., & Damasio, H. (2018). Music training and child development: a review of recent findings from a longitudinal study. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 0(0).
  20. Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior, function, and structure. Neuron, 76(3), 486-502.
  21. Holtzer, R., Friedman, R., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M., Xue, X., & Verghese, J. (2007). The relationship between specific cognitive functions and falls in aging. Neuropsychology, 21(5), 540.
  22. Jahan, S. (2017). Human Development Report 2016-Human Development for Everyone (No. id: 12021).
  23. Jaschke, A. C., Honing, H., & Scherder, E. J. (2018). Longitudinal analysis of music education on executive functions in primary school children. Frontiers in neuroscience, 12, 103.
  24. Jauset-Berrocal, J.A., Tripovic, Y., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J., (2014). El método BAPNE y su repercusión en las capacidades cognitivas. XII Jornadas de Redes de Investigación en Docencia Universitaria. Universidad de Alicante.
  25. Jiménez-Molina, J. B., Vicedo-Cantó, E., Sayago-Martínez, R., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Evaluating attention, socioemotional factors and anxiety in secondary school students in Murcia (Spain) using the BAPNE® Method. Research protocol. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1071-1075.
  26. Kokotsaki, D., & Hallam, S. (2007). Higher education music students’ perceptions of the benefits of participative music making. Music Education Research, 9(1), 93-109.
  27. Koppitz, E. M (2006). El dibujo de la figura humana en los niños. Evaluación psicológica. Argentina: Editorial Guadalupe.
  28. Kraus, N., Slater, J., Thompson, E. C., Hornickel, J., Strait, D. L., Nicol, T., & White-Schwoch, T. (2014). Music enrichment programs improve the neural encoding of speech in at-risk children. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(36), 11913-11918.
  29. Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143-148.
  30. Littlewood, P., Glorieux, I., & Jönsson, I. (2017). Social exclusion in Europe: problems and paradigms. Taylor & Francis.
  31. Marcuzzi, C., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). BAPNE Method, Developmental Dyslexia and Inclusive Education: Cognitive, Socio-Emotional and Psychomotor Stimulation in Secondary School. A Practical Resource for Education within a Cross Curriculum. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1291-1298.
  32. McPhillips, M., & Jordan-Black, J. A. (2007). The effect of social disadvantage on motor development in young children: a comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 1214-1222.
  33. Moral-Bofill, L., Romero Naranjo, F. J., Albiar-Aliaga, E., & Cid-Lamas, J. A. (2015). The BAPNE Method as a School Intervention and Support Strategy to Improve the School Environment and Contribute to Socioemotional Learning (SEL).
  34. OECD (2011). Promoting child development and child well-being, in Doing better for families. Paris: OECD.
  35. Piek, J. P., Dyck, M. J., Nieman, A., Anderson, M., Hay, D., Smith, L. M., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). The relationship between motor coordination, executive functioning and attention in school aged children. Archives of clinical Neuropsychology, 19(8), 1063-1076.
  36. Pons-Terrés, J. M., Romero-Naranjo, A. A., Romero-Naranjo, F. J., Crespo-Colomino, N., & Liendo-Cárdenas, A. (2014). Estimulación de la atención dividida: Didáctica de la Percusión Corporal – Método BAPNE. XII Jornadas de Redes de Investigación en Docencia Universitaria. Universidad de Alicante
  37. Presti, G., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). Singing Canons: A didactic suggestion, using BAPNE Method. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1449-1454.
  38. Rauscher, F. H., & Hinton, S. C. (2006). The Mozart effect: Music listening is not music instruction. Educational Psychologist, 41(4), 233-238.
  39. Romero Naranjo, F. J., Summer, J., & Palencia Pérez, F. (2012). Body percussion: theoretical practical foundation = fundamentación teórico práctica: BAPNE. Barcelona: Body music Body percussion.
  40. Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2013). Science & art of body percussion: a review. Journal of human sport and exercise, 8, 442-457. Retrieved from: http://hdl.handle.net/10045/29740
  41. Romero-Naranjo, F. J., & Naranjo, A. A. R. (2013). La percusión corporal como recurso terapéutico. In XI Jornadas de Redes de Investigación en Docencia Universitaria: Retos de futuro en la enseñanza superior (pp. 1734-1747). Instituto de Ciencias de la Educación.
  42. Romero-Naranjo, F. J., Crespo-Colomino, N., Liendo-Cárdenas, A., Pons-Terrés, J., & Carretero-Martínez, A. (2014). Drugs and Body Percussion: Rehabilitation Therapy Using the BAPNE Method. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 152, 1282-1287.
  43. Ron, D. (2013). Child poverty and social exclusion: A framework for European action. library Briefing. Library of the European Parliament
  44. Sachs, M., Kaplan, J., Sarkissian, A. D., & Habibi, A. (2017). Increased engagement of the cognitive control network associated with music training in children during an fMRI Stroop task. PLOS ONE, 12(10), e0187254.
  45. Saegert, S. C., Adler, N. E., Bullock, H. E., Cauce, A. M., Liu, W. M., & Wyche, K. F. (2007). Report of the APA task force on socioeconomic status. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  46. Salerno, G., Cefaratti, L., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2017). The BAPNE Method: A New Approach and Treatment for Depressive Disorders. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 237, 1439-1443. DOI:
  47. Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience & Bio behavioral Reviews, 24(1), 7-12.
  48. Sheehy-Skeffington, J., & Rea, J. (2017). How Poverty Affects People's Decision-making Processes. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
  49. Sneiderman, S. (2011). Consideraciones acerca de la confiabilidad y validez en las técnicas proyectivas. Subjetividad y procesos cognitivos, 15(2), 93-110.
  50. Statistics Explained. (2015). People at risk of poverty or social exclusion. EUROSTAT.
  51. Sulkin, I., & Brodsky, W. (2007). The effects of hand-clapping songs training on temporal-motor skills among elementary school children. In K. Overy (Ed.), Proceedings of the Summer Workshop on Music, Language, and Movement, 2007. Edinburgh: Institute for Music in Human and Social Development, University of Edinburgh.
  52. Tierney, A. T., Krizman, J., & Kraus, N. (2015). Music training alters the course of adolescent auditory development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(32), 10062-10067.
  53. Trives-Martínez, E. A. T., Naranjo, F. J. R., Terrés, J. M. P., Naranjo, A. A. R., Colomino, N. C., Cárdenas, A. L., & Tripovic, Y. (2014). Los métodos didáctico musicales y la atención en relación al movimiento. In XII Jornadas de redes de investigación en docencia universitaria. El reconocimiento docente: Innovar e investigar con criterios de calidad (pp. 1066-1079). Instituto de Ciencias de la Educación.
  54. Vauhkonen, T., Kallio, J., Kauppinen, T. M., & Erola, J. (2017). Intergenerational accumulation of social disadvantages across generations in young adulthood. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 48, 42-52.
  55. Walper, S. (2009). Links of perceived economic deprivation to adolescents’ well-being six years later. ZfF–Zeitschrift für Familienforschung. Journal of Family Research, 21(2).
  56. Younge, T. B. (2013). Socioeconomic Status, Family Environment, and Executive Functions During Middle Childhood (Doctoral dissertation). University of California, Berkeley.

Copyright information

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

About this article

Publication Date

09 April 2019

eBook ISBN

978-1-80296-059-4

Publisher

Future Academy

Volume

60

Print ISBN (optional)

-

Edition Number

1st Edition

Pages

1-1062

Subjects

Multicultural education, education, personal health, public health, social discrimination,social inequality

Cite this article as:

Latre-Nava, S., Martínez-Fernández, X., Rodríguez-Masafrets, A., Puigdevall-Cayuela, A., Torre, G., Finestres-Alberola, J., & Romero-Naranjo, F. J. (2019). Cognitive Stimulation In Adolescents At Risk Of Exclusion Using The BAPNE® Method. In E. Soriano, C. Sleeter, M. Antonia Casanova, R. M. Zapata, & V. C. Cala (Eds.), The Value of Education and Health for a Global, Transcultural World, vol 60. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 489-499). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2019.04.02.62