Spontaneous Play As An Indicator Of Children’s School Readiness In Social Skills

Abstract

Starting compulsory education represents a key milestone in children’s lives. A successful start in the system of compulsory education is affected by the level of children’s school maturity and readiness. This is influenced especially by the family but also by preschool education in which teachers systematically focus on children’s development in the cognitive, psychomotoric and social skills. Between 2009 and 2013, a research team at the Faculty of Education, Palacký University in Olomouc, Czech Republic, performed a research study aimed at school maturity with an emphasis on cognitive maturity. This research was followed by another study aimed at children’s readiness in the area of social skills. The research is of a quantitative-qualitative nature; the principal research method was observation and interview. The purpose of the paper is to present the partial results of an analysis of video-studies aimed at children’s spontaneous play in the kindergarten. The authors analysed and assessed the main areas and indicators of children’s play with a focus on social skills. In the context of the preschool curriculum, the authors formulated conclusions and recommendations not only in terms of further research but also for preschool education.

Keywords: Preschool childplayeducationsocial skills

Introduction

With the introduction of the last obligatory year of preschool education in the Czech Republic starting in 2018, systematic preparation of children with the aim to make them able to start compulsory schooling is becoming more important. This fact is directly linked to a greater emphasis on pedagogical diagnosis by means of which the teacher assesses the current state of childrenʼs development and, in direct relationship, develops step by step the childrenʼs competencies. For the purposes of pedagogical diagnostics, teachers mostly use controlled activities. Our current research shows that play, especially spontaneous one, is only used by teachers a little as a means of pedagogical diagnostics. Although observation is the main method of pedagogical diagnostics, we realize that it is a very demanding method that requires the ability to observe, analyze and draw conclusions. It is often less graspable for teachers than an analysis of a work assignment for example, where the teacher can clearly decide whether or not a child has managed a task. We do not want to underestimate the pedagogical diagnostics of managed activities in any way; what we want to draw attention to are the opportunities offered by the analysis of childrenʼs play.

It should not be forgotten though that children are absolutely spontaneous in terms of their emotional, social, cognitive and psychomotoric skills. One part of the research we have been carrying out since 2017 is to create and validate a tool for pedagogical diagnostics of school preparedness based on the method of spontaneous game observation. The aim of our contribution is to make public the results of our research in which we verified the tool for pedagogical diagnostics of school preparedness with a focus on childrenʼs social skills in the frame of spontaneous games.

Play as childrenʼs most natural activity

The childrenʼs development is affected by both internal and external factors. By external factors, we mean the closest neighborhood in which the child learns, imitates, takes on patterns of behaviour, has new experiences, satisfies his/her own needs (physical, mental and social), etc. In the environment the child lives in, he/she interacts with other people, the most important ones being the family, i.e. the mother, father, brothers or sisters, grandparents. Gradually, this circle extends to children of the same age, teachers and other people. Play is an essential and very important activity in a childʼs life, in particular an individual game in which an adult is the game partner but also a play in a group of children of the same age. The game is based on the needs of the child where his/her inner motivation plays an important role rather than external stimuli. Spontaneity is one of the basic characteristics of childrenʼs play. The child does not follow personal goals in the game. The game is also characterized by symbolism when a child brings into the game his/her own experiences, the ideas he/she draws from his/her surroundings. (Šmelová, 2014) The conditions of the game, into which we count not only the quality of material equipment but also people, significantly influence the overall development of childrenʼs personality which also means the ​​social skills that are the subject of our research.

Curriculum for preschool education

The curriculum document called “The Framework Educational Program for Preschool Education” (hereinafter referred to as RVP PV) is currently in force in the Czech Republic, starting in 2018. This document is binding for all kindergartens registered in the registry of the Ministry of Education of the Czech Republic. It represents a generally valid framework under which kindergartens perform preschool education. It includes framework objectives, key competences, educational content as well as educational conditions. It also contains and recommends appropriate educational means, methods and organizational forms suitable for preschool education. Key competences are sets of supposed skills, knowledge, attitudes and habits that a child who completes preschool education is generally able to achieve. These competences represent the outputs of preschool education which is directly followed by primary school education. Monitoring of the level of key competencies constitutes one of everyday diagnostic activities of the teachers. The level of these can be monitored in all the activities of the child during the day. The curriculum focuses quite considerably on social skills. It defines communicative, problem solving, social and personal, activity-related and civic competencies. All competencies are interconnected and linked to one another. (RVP PV, 2018)

Problem Statement

At present, the most up-to-date topics include the already mentioned preparation of children to start compulsory schooling and improving its quality. The necessity of finding the solution is related not only to the persistent high percentage of postponement of compulsory school attendance (approx. 15 %) in the Czech Republic, but also to the introduction of the final compulsory year of preschool education and its alternative to individual preschool education. As shown by our research (Šmelová et al., 2008 till present day), as well as by the results of the surveys carried out by the Czech School Inspection Bureau, kindergartens are devoted to preparing children to enter primary schools but they often prepare them with low efficiency. In this context, problems have been identified in the area of ​​pedagogical diagnostics and the use of a properly defined diagnosis aimed at achieving the key competencies of the child that are necessary for the successful start of compulsory schooling.

For the purposes of our research, we have identified a basic topic (problem), i.e. pedagogical diagnostics of selected social skills of children in spontaneous games in kindergartens before starting compulsory schooling.

Research Questions

The research questions are determined in the context of the topic being researched. The first research question relates to the identification of social skills in terms of school preparedness in the context of a preschool curriculum, focusing on self-discipline within imaginative games. The second research question was determined in the context of the expected outcomes, i.e. the criteria of school preparedness focusing on social skills.

RQ1: Are children able to conform to the rules of others, to give up their projections of something for the benefit of other children when playing spontaneous games?

Partial questions:

  • How does childrenʼs self-discipline in the analyzed games manifest itself? Are children able to adopt a different role in the game in order to maintain cooperation?

  • What kinds occur within the analyzed spontaneous games?

RQ2: Can selected social skills based on established criteria be diagnosed within analyzed spontaneous games of children aged 5–7?

Purpose of the Study

The aim of this study was

  • to describe and characterize a childʼs game in the environment of a kindergarten by means of an analysis of video recordings

  • to analyze and evaluate the main areas as well as indicators of childrenʼs play with a focus on their social skills.

  • to identify and evaluate the childrenʼs social skills in relation to their readiness to start compulsory school attendance

Research Methods

Methods of data collection: indirect observation by means of a video taken in selected kindergartens, evaluation by means of created record sheets with pre-defined criteria of school preparedness.

Method of selecting participants of the research: criteria-based selection (criteria: kindergarten located within the city of Olomouc, child 5–7 years old, consent of the parents of the child, the childʼs attendance in the kindergarten taking place in early morning hours.)

Video capture period: April – June 2018.

Explored participants: 5 girls and 5 boys aged 5–7

Findings

Social competences represent an important area of school preparedness. Preschool education should provide a variety of situations for their development. It is spontaneous play that has a great potential and that we are focusing on in our research.

Evaluation of Research Question No. 1

RQ1: Are children able to conform to the rules of others, to give up their projections of something for the benefit of other children when playing spontaneous games?

Partial questions:

  • What kinds occur within the analyzed spontaneous games?

  • How does childrenʼs self-discipline in the analyzed games manifest itself? Are children able to adopt a different role in the game in order to maintain cooperation?

The games observed in the analyzed videos can be divided into two groups, namely table games and carpet games. When playing table games, children drew, painted, used constructive games, did jigsaw puzzles. Most often, these were individual games. There only were two exceptions, two girls who cooperated in painting the subject defined by the teacher and then boys who played with dice. The second group consisted of children playing games on the carpet. The most common were social games, namely household life, transport, soldiers. The second most common type of games were the constructive ones when children “built” houses, roads or means of transport. To a lesser extent, motion games were recorded using rhythmic instruments. Children most often played in pairs, with rarely appearing groups of 3 to 5 children. According to Millar (1978), children aged 5 are able to play for a longer period in a group of four to five children. This fact is confirmed by our analysis, too. As far as the choice of a game partner is concerned, most of the monitored time, children played with the same friend, even when they started playing a different game. Furthermore, our research corresponds to Millarʼs statement that brothers and sisters pay attention to each other and are able to cooperate much earlier, as evidenced by the analysis of the play of two pairs of siblings (2-3, 6-7 years old). Self-discipline is an important part of social skills. In this context, we observed the children in order to find out whether a particular child is able to comply with the rules or take on a different role within the games, based on their internal motivation. It is possible to note that girls in particular managed to agree on something with one another. When there was a mixed group of both girls and boys, the girls were more proactive in giving proposals and promoting these. Problems with cooperation were more common in boys. If they were not happy with the game, they broke it down (for example, they broke a construction being built) or switched to another game. More frequent minor conflicts were also found more often in boys, especially in the case of toy disputes. The teachersʼ interventions into observed games were minimal and rather aiming to make the children follow the discipline or in order to make a game finish and simultaneously motivate the children to go on to play another game. It was obvious that children are used to playing without the support of teachers because we did not notice that children would ever require their help. Bruner (1980) reports the results of a preschool research project having taken place in Oxford (Oxford preschool research project), in which it was found out that the higher the ratio of the number of adults to the number of children in the group, the higher the likelihood of a game to be more sophisticated. (In Bruce, 1996, p. 53) If we evaluate the extent of the sophistiated character of games in terms of their contents, we may note that the games were poorly developed, which usually led to their end after quite a short time. It was evident that the children “did not know” how to proceed in the game. Teachers entered the games in the role of a game partner minimally. The level of observed games supports the findings of the above research. All that we have mentioned so far shows that teachers do not give enough estimation to their role of game partners through which they might be able to approach children closer, to understand the childrenʼs world but also to encourage their play with the goals of developing the childrenʼs potential.

Evaluation of Research Question No. 2

RQ2: Can selected social skills based on established criteria be diagnosed within analyzed spontaneous games (in children 5–7 years of age)?

Table 1 -
See Full Size >

Followed criteria in social skills

In connection with this research question, we formulated some criteria in accordance with the RVP PV curriculum document. We verified the criteria we had determined using the methodologies chosen.

The criteria were assessed using a three-point scale. Each child was evaluated separately, the data was recorded in record sheets and commented on. By means of a content analysis of the transcribed text, we consequently draw conclusions.

Table 2 -
See Full Size >
Table 3 -
See Full Size >

Partial conclusions:

In girls, some problems have appeared with listening to others, collaborating with them and respecting their needs. We find the findings in Girl No. 1 particularly interesting. The dominant tendencies were identified with this girl, especially when playing with younger children. Her manipulation with them showed clearly that this girl satisfied her needs regardless of those of the other children. Nevertheless, she was kind and nice to them. Potential problems may arise after the girl enters the first year of the primary school, as the whole class will be the same age. When analyzing the criteria for Girl No. 2, there was a risk of early entry into the primary school starting with the beginning of the following school year. As shown in the specialited literature (Čížková et al., 1999; Šmelová, Petrová, Souralová et al., 2012), the girl shows signs of lack of preparedness. She does not join group activities and if she does, then rather in a subordinate role. It was obvious that he was not a sought-after partner for the other children. Her behaviour showed lower self-confidence, lower self-esteem and inability to establish communication with other children. The girl therefore devoted herself to individual activities or looked for the teacherʼs company. It may be noted that the postponement of compulsory school attendance would be a benefit for this girl. Admission to the first year of the primary school is a major change in the childʼs life also in the social sphere. As a result of her lack of preparedness to change the school group, to start the role of a schoolchild, etc., the girl may fall behind in other areas such as cognitive or biological development, too.

The analysis of the boysʼ play showed no significant problem. There were a few of situations in which the boys had to agree on something in order to solve the situation, which they eventually managed doing without much difficulty. The boys did not show any dominant or agressive behavious to themselves or others. They were able to join the other childrenʼs games as well as to lead the game, they showed empathy. The boy No. 4 who kept having his favourite cuddly toy nearby most of the day is worth mentioning. For boys, the presence of their toy may constitute an assuring element and remind them of their home. At this age, we do not consider this fact as negative.

In order to compare the two groups, we may note we have observed a potential risk of early entrance of the primary school education in Girl No. 2 and Girl No. 1 where we have seen a certain lack of social maturity in the context of playing with younger children. In girls, we noted a higher number of games linked to family life, e.g. playing a game imitating everyday hosehold life, which is definitely linked to their future feminine roles. On the other hand, we have noted no potential risk of early entrance of the primary school education in boys. It is likely that this finding is related to the fact that most boys observed have been granted a postponement of their compulsory schooling last year. Therefore, they had plenty of time to mature in their skills, including the social ones.

Summary of RQ2: In the frame of the spontaneous game of children, we were able to capture and evaluate the defined criteria being in line with the RVP PV as indicators of school preparedness in the area of the childrenʼs social skills. Spontaneous game analysis is an exceptional opportunity for pedagogical diagnosis of childrenʼs readiness to enter the primary school. The fact that the child is authentic and relaxed in spontaneous games is a big plus, the level of his/her skills in all aspects of his/her development being reflected in the game. We believe that teachers should pay more attention to the observation of spontaneous play of children. Thus, they are able to get valuable information for pedagogical diagnostics and, at the same time, they can systematically develop the child by means of appropriate playful activities.

Conclusion

If an adult (a teacher in our case) is supposed to understand children, to know their individual possibilities and needs and to apply an appropriate educational strategy, they have to master the basic method of pedagogical diagnostics, i.e. observation, and supplement this with other methods of pedagogical diagnostics. A spontaneous game is an activity where the teacher can monitor the progress of children and simultaneously use the game as a means of preparing children to start primary education schooling by creating a stimulating environment. The kindergarten teacher should perform the pedagogical diagnostics systematically throughout the day, i.e. in controlled and spontaneous activities.

Reinforcement of the pedagogical diagnostics in the context of preschool pedagogy and developmental psychology proves to be essential both in undergraduate training of future kindergarten teachers and in further professional training of this professional group.

Acknowledgments

The paper was supported by the following project: ‘Play as a means of preparation of the child for compulsory education’ IGA_PdF_2018_017

References

  1. Bruce, T. (1996). Předškolní výchova: deset principů moderní pedagogiky a jejich aplikace v praxi. Praha: Portál.
  2. Čížková, J. et al. (1999). Přehled vývojové psychologie. Olomouc: Univerzita Palackého
  3. Millar, S. (1978). Psychologie hry. Praha: Panorama.
  4. Rámcový vzdělávací program pro předškolní vzdělávání / RVP PV. (2018). Praha: MŠMT
  5. Šmelová, E., Petrová, A, Souralová, E. et al., (2012). Pre-school education in the context of curriculum:children's readiness for compulsory school attendance in the context of selected EU countries – Czech Republic, Slovakia, Slovenia, Poland. 1st English ed. Olomouc: Palacký University Olomouc.
  6. Šmelová, E. (2014). Bezvýhradná akceptace ve výchově dítěte. Olomouc: Univerzita Palackého v Olomouci.

Copyright information

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

About this article

Publication Date

14 January 2019

eBook ISBN

978-1-80296-052-5

Publisher

Future Academy

Volume

53

Print ISBN (optional)

-

Edition Number

1st Edition

Pages

1-812

Subjects

Education, educational psychology, counselling psychology

Cite this article as:

Šmelová, E., & Berčíková, A. (2019). Spontaneous Play As An Indicator Of Children’s School Readiness In Social Skills. In Z. Bekirogullari, M. Y. Minas, & R. X. Thambusamy (Eds.), ICEEPSY 2018: Education and Educational Psychology, vol 53. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 476-485). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2019.01.45