Abstract
Gender binoms provide information on cultural value as they reflect typical of perception of reality in different societies. The object of this research is gender binoms such as a man and a woman, a daughter and a son. This paper aims to study the structure of gender binoms of family kinship as a semiotic means of gender-related cultural concepts. The article presents the results of the linguistic experiment of a cross-cultural comparison of gender binoms. In this experiment, the corpus-driven approach to Google search was used to collect the empirical data. This technique was shown to be an effective way of determining the dominating elements of gender binoms in two cultures. A mechanism of comparing these dominating elements is proposed, based on observations from this experiment. The study proves that the change in the structure of a gender binom shows the difference in the cultural perceptions of the male and female nominations. The issue highlighted in this study may be applicable to cognitive grammar and semantics research, to sociolinguistics and cultural studies.
Keywords: Gender binomscross cultural studies
Introduction
Gender is a social construct, a set of characteristics defined by the culture of a society that identifies the social behavior of men and women and the relationship between them. Swedish researcher Yvonne Hirdman (1991) considers the gender system a set of relations between men and women, including representations, informal and formal rules and norms defined by the place, goals, and perception of the sexes in society. She describes the gender system as a set of gender contracts (Hirdman, 1991, p. 190).
Linguists have tackled almost all areas of language from a gender perspective. This research area proposes cognitive-pragmatic and stylistic models of gender description and investigates the gender asymmetries in language (Kamenskaya, 2002; Kiesling, 2019; Goroshko, 2003; Meyerhoff & Holmes, 2003; Talbot, 2019).
The motivations for this research and its importance stem from several existing studies.
First, there are cognitive studies of various mechanisms for implementing gender procedures, for example, the child’s awareness of gender differences (Martin, 2012), the theories of gender development (Bussey & Bandura, 1999; Martin, 2012). According to cognitive theorists, individuals are active information processors and prior expectations and cognitions play an important role in how incoming information stimuli available to the senses is arranged and processed. The information available to a recipient is not constrained by the abilities to process and deal with information from that world. Cognitive theorists often gain insights into information processing by assessing misperceptions, inaccurate memories, use of heuristics, and selective attention and memory, because in those instances there may be incongruence between the environmental input and one’s concepts (Bussey & Bandura, 1999; Kirschner et al., 2017; Martin, 2012; Resnick, 2017; Schunk & Usher, 2019).
Second, cognitive linguistics studies of gender cover various aspects, such as language construction of knowledge about gender, the conceptualization of speech behavior, metaphor and gender in the processes of categorization and conceptualization (Gritsenko, 2011; 2016; Shatunova, 2018; Chernykh, 2018). Many gender cognitive studies focused on these processes using the nominative resources of the language, however, the role of semiotic means and deep (structural) mechanisms of language and discourse still requires comprehensive investigation. Therefore, it is important to fill this gap in the literature by researching gender binoms from the perspective of the language sign.
Problem Statement
Scientists in cognitive semantics Popova and Sternin (2005) offered a set of postulates to use while researching the relationship of language semantics with the concept sphere of people and semantic processes. Here are some of them: the term “concept” belongs to human consciousness, it is a global unit of thought activity, a quantum of structured knowledge. Human thinking is nonverbal, it is carried out by using a universal object code. People think in concepts encoded by units of this code. An ordered set of concepts in the human mind forms its concept sphere (Popova & Sternin, 2005, p. 7).
As stated by the studies of gender conceptualization, the gender propositions are not semantically proxy in the conceptualizing gender relationships, they correlate with a single center that corresponds with the concepts of femininity (masculinity) specific to a culture. This method of categorization represents radial models comprising a central term and variants. The relationship between the central terminal and variants is conventional, and an individual assimilates it in the process of socialization. Specifics of masculinity and femininity as radial conceptual categories is that their central members do not exist outside of their manifestations (variants) (Gritsenko, 2011, 2016). Therefore, gender nominations perform not only a nominative function, but they translate the conceptual meaning in the binary opposition
Gender binoms act as syntactic units related to cognitive modeling of reality. This is due to the following provisions: Norman (2013) treats syntactic models as a tool of cognition. The basic premise is that grammatical unit of a language “store in a concentrated form the cognitive experience of previous generations” and “allow the native speaker to organize, bring into the system new, just received information” (Norman, 2013, p. 34). Analyzing the principles of syntactic model identification in the cognitive paradigm, he emphasizes that we can consider the syntactic model a component of the cognition process only if this model and its components are “endowed with some generalized meaning” (Norman, 2013, p. 38). According to the author, the syntactic model naturally reflects reality, fixing a certain typical situation, which is the plan of its content. By fixing an event, the syntactic model imposes several quantitative and qualitative restrictions on it. Quantitative restrictions mean the choice of a certain small number of “participants” (people, objects, places or events, etc.), and qualitative restrictions mean a certain content with a set of participants (Norman, 2013).
In this paper, we consider a syntactic structure of gender binoms from the perspective of iconicity (Fischer, 1997; Haiman, 1983; 1985), which shows a relationship between the two sides of the sign. A language sign constructed on an iconic principle reflects the speaker’s choice of a language sign isomorphic to the mental representation of the object in the speaker’s mind. The structure of an iconic sign implemented through a gender binom is the relator structure. Van Langendonck (2007) defines a relator as a free or bound morpheme that has two syntagmatic slots (p. 410). In the relator structure, the relator defines a specific semantic-syntactic relationship between the two relata. In gender binoms, the relata are represented by two elements:
Research Questions
The research questions of the study are:
(1) what is the difference in the implementation of gender cultural concepts through binominal structures in Russian and English?
(2) can Google search provide relevant results in the context of the gender binominal structures studies?
Purpose of the Study
This paper aims to study the binomial gender structures as a semiotic means of realization of gender concepts of culture. The object of this research is nominative phrases such as
The article presents the results of a cross-cultural comparison of gender binominal phrases using the resources provided by modern technologies, in particular the Google search engine. The present study compares these units in two languages to determine the dominating elements of the binoms in two cultures.
We argue that gender binoms are the language implementation of cultural concepts associated with gender. They are language sign structures built on the principle of a schematic iconicity of distance, which means that the first element occupies a dominant position in the concept sphere of the speaker, while the second one is less significant. The positional change in gender binoms such as
This study sought to:
1. establish the status of gender binoms in the translation of cultural concepts;
2. develop a method for researching into cross-cultural comparison of gender binoms based on the corpus approach using Google tools;
3. compare relevant gender binoms in English and Russian.
Research Methods
To collect the empirical data of the study, we conducted a linguistic experiment based on the corpus approach with the use of Google. This method was not previously applied in cognitive studies of gender binoms, however, it has already been used in several modern cognitive research (Petrova, 2019; Suleimanova, 2019; Suleimanova & Petrova, 2020).
In this experiment, we studied 16 Russian and English gender binoms. 8 binoms had the structure:
The protocol of the experiment had three stages:
Stage 1. Google search for gender binoms as Phrase 1 and Phrase 2.
Stage 2. Analysis of the number of occurrences in Google of Phrase 1 and Phrase 2.
Stage 3. Calculation of the percentage of Phrase 2 to Phrase 1.
The reasoning for the calculation was:
Phrase 1 is the original word combination with 100% stabile structure.
Phrase 2 is a changed version of the stable structure.
the percentage of occurrences of Phrase 2 against Phrase 1 determines the proportion of the changed phrase regarding the original one. To calculate the percentage, we use the formula: Phrase 2 x% = n2: n1 x100,
where n1 is the number of occurrences of Phrase 1 in Google search and n2 is the number of occurrences of Phrase 2.
We can visualize this results in the diagram of percentage ratio of Phrase 1 and Phrase 2 (Figure
The percentage of Phase 1 and Phrase 2 shows the ratio of the variability of the structure.
The comparison of the ratio of the structural variability in the two languages displays which element of the cultural concepts is more stable and, which tends to changeability in the English- and Russian-speaking segments of the Internet. The data accumulated with the help of Google can be analyzed according to the described scheme. The analysis allows for concluding the stability of the relator structure in two languages, which indicates the changeability of the cultural concepts
Findings
Table
The percentage of gender binom variation shows how flexible the concepts
The difference in the variation of the binom
The percentage of Phrases 1 and 2 of the binom
The binom
In the binom
In Russian the binom
The comparison of the binom
The dynamics of the variability of concepts in the binom
An interesting observation can be made while studying the binom
Conclusion
The results presented in this paper provide several findings that enhance our understanding of the role of the structure of gender binoms and its correlation with the culture.
First, gender binoms reflecting the cultural concepts of
Second, due to the iconic nature of the language sign, the arrangement of the gender binom elements (or relata) can accentuate the dominating element of the opposition.
Third, changing the position of elements and analyzing the empirical data, we can establish the changeability of gender binoms in different languages.
Fourth, the Russian-speaking culture tends more towards masculine forms in gender binoms, in the English-speaking culture there is a relative parity. However, the use of the conceptual structure
The results of the experiment and the research methods of the study, however, require further expansion and clarification because of the limitations of the specified search query of binoms in pdf format provided by Google. These limitations relate to the methodology which can further elaborated in research perspective. But the model of the experiment proved to be effective for conducting cross-cultural research providing relevant data. With the currently available data, we presented results that are in line with the study’s hypothesis: the positional change in gender binoms such as man and woman (1) vs woman and man (2) reveals the change in the cognitive focus of the speaker. Further discoveries in the text realizations of the variability of gender binom structure may clarify the differentiation in the results of this experiment.
References
- Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological review, 106(4), 676-713.
- Chernykh, O. Y. (2018). Lingvisticheskoe konstruirovanie gendera [Linguistic construction of gender]. Mir nauki, kul'tury, obrazovaniya, 4(71), 562-565.
- Fischer, O. (1997). Iconicity in language and literature: language innovation and language change. Neuphilologische Mitteilungen, 98, 63-87. https://www.jstor.org/stable/43346409
- Givón, T. (1991). Isomorphism in the grammatical code: cognitive and biological considerations. Studies in language, 15(1), 85-114.
- Goroshko, E. I. (2003). Yazykovoe soznanie: gendernaya paradigma [Language consciousness: gender paradigm]. In Metodologiya sovremennoj psiholingvistiki (pp. 3-12). Publishing house of the Altai University.
- Gritsenko, E. S. (2011). Metafora i gender v processah kategorizacii i konceptualizacii [Metaphor and gender in the processes of categorization and conceptualization. Voprosy kognitivnoj lingvistiki, 2(27), 13-17.
- Gritsenko, E. S. (2016). Gender i stil': konceptualizaciya osobennostej rechevogo povedeniya [Gender and style: conceptualization of speech behavior features. Kognitivnye issledovaniya yazyka, 24, 216-224.
- Haiman, J. (1983). Iconic and economic motivation. Language, 59(4), 781-819. https://doi.org/10.2307/413373
- Haiman, J. (1985). Natural syntax. Iconicity and erosion. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics London, 44, 1-285.
- Hirdman, Y. (1991). The gender system. In T. Andreasen, & Y. Hirdman (Eds.), Moving on: new perspectives on the Women's movement (pp. 187-207). University Press.
- Kamenskaya, O. L. (2002). Gendergetika – nauka budushchego [Gender studies – science of the future]. In Gender kak intriga poznaniya (pp. 13-19). Rudomino.
- Kiesling, S. F. (2019). Language, gender, and sexuality: an introduction. Routledge.
- Kirschner, P. A., Park, B., Malone, S., & Jarodzka, H. (2017). Toward a cognitive theory of multimedia assessment (CTMMA). In Learning, design, and technology: an international compendium of theory, research, practice, and policy (pp. 1-23). Springer, Cham. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/85227369.pdf
- Langacker, R. W. (2009). Investigations in cognitive grammar. Vol. 42. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
- Martin, C. L. (2012). Cognitive theories of gender development. In The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 105-136). Psychology Press.
- Meyerhoff, M., & Holmes, J. (2003). The handbook of language and gender. Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
- Norman, B. Y. (2013). Kognitivnyj sintaksis russkogo yazyka [Cognitive syntax of the Russian language]. FlintA.
- Petrova, I. M. (2019). Potencial poiskovoj sistemy Google pri provedenii issledovanij v ramkah kognitivnoj korpusnoj lingvistiki [The potential of google search for studies in cognitive corpus linguistics]. Teoreticheskaya i prikladnaya lingvistika, 5(3), 127-142. https://doi.org/10.22250/2410-7190_2019_5_3_127_142
- Popova, Z. D., & Sternin, I. A. (2005). Osnovnye cherty semantiko-kognitivnogo podhoda k yazyku [The main features of the semantic-cognitive approach to language]. In V. I. Karasik, I. A. Sternin (Eds.), Antologiya contseptov, (pp. 7-10). Volgograd: Paradigma.
- Resnick, L. B. (2017). Toward a cognitive theory of instruction. In Learning and motivation in the classroom (pp. 5-38). Routledge.
- Schunk, D. H., & Usher, E. L. (2019). Social cognitive theory and motivation. In The Oxford handbook of human motivation. Oxford University Press.
- Shatunova, G. A. (2018). Л Leksicheskaya reprezentaciya gendernyh stereotipov v britanskoj presse XX veka [Lexical representation of gender stereotypes in the British press of the early twentieth century]. Vestnik Moskovskogo universiteta. Seriya: Filologiya. Teoriya yazyka. Yazykovoe obrazovanie, 1(29), 107-111.
- Suleimanova, О. A., & Petrova, I. M. (2020). Using big-data experiments in cognitive and linguo-cultural research in English and Russian. Journal of Siberian Federal University. Humanities & Social Sciences, 13(3), 392–400. https://doi.org/10.17516/1997-1370-0561
- Suleimanova, O. A. (2019). Semanticheskij eksperiment: novye vozmozhnosti v sisteme koordinat Big Data [A semantic experiment: new opportunities in the big data coordination system]. Kognitivnye issledovaniya yazyka, 36, 427-432.
- Talbot, M. (2019). Language and gender. John Wiley & Sons.
- Van Langendonck, W. (2007). Iconicity. In D. Geeraerts, & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738632.013.0016
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
20 November 2020
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-094-5
Publisher
European Publisher
Volume
95
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-1241
Subjects
Sociolinguistics, discourse analysis, bilingualism, multilingualism
Cite this article as:
Petrova, I. M. (2020). Using Google Tools For Cross-Cultural Comparison Of Gender Binoms In English And Russian. In Е. Tareva, & T. N. Bokova (Eds.), Dialogue of Cultures - Culture of Dialogue: from Conflicting to Understanding, vol 95. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 723-731). European Publisher. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2020.11.03.77