Narratives Of Which Modality Are The Most Accessible For Younger Students’ Understanding?

Abstract

The article is devoted to the issue of the accessibility of understanding polycode narratives for students in primary grades. It is shown that narrative texts encountered in the experience of younger schoolchildren can be presented both in the traditional linguistic (printed) form and as a visual narrative (comics, graphic stories). The peculiarities of perception and understanding of visual narrative are related to the configuration of visual information, which is important for the selection of polycode texts as a didactic tool. Such texts are especially effective for the development of the textual competence of elementary schoolchildren at the initial stage of education but can be integrated into educational material during the entire period of study at the stage of primary general education. According to the survey the success of primary school students in understanding a vector-type polycode narrative text was found. Gender differences while working with various types of narratives were revealed: girls demonstrate great success in working with polycode narratives and boys – with text narratives.

Keywords: Comicspolycode textreadingunderstanding narratives

Introduction

The tendency of the development of modern education is caused by the transition to a new format of educational space with a multimodal nature. A large role in its equipment is played by visual practices, multimodal and polycode means. The latter, among other things, include polycode texts – “texts in which the language code is combined with codes of a completely different semiotic system (visual, acoustic, kinesthetic, etc.). Moreover, the non-linguistic part carries an independent but dependent on the linguistic information load” (Shulekina, 2018).

Research in the field of psycholinguistics (Cohn, 2013; Dunst et al., 2018) indicates the differential feature of such texts – the asymmetry of the information which is contained in them. It means that instead of linear way of interpretation of the content by means of a system of linguistic signs, as it occurs in a traditional language text, here the content is mediated by the interaction of the code structures of the text, some of which may prevail and even suppress others depending on the context. That is why the mechanisms of semantic perception differ in comparison with the perception of a traditional text during reading. Otherwise the integrative work of cognitive functions is included in the processing of sign information (Brenna, 2012; Martín-Arnala et al., 2019; Schnotz, 2014). Despite the complexity of the interaction of the codes, they retain the main signs of the semantic integrity of the text as a language unit – an optional title; the division into paragraphs, which is provided, for example, by modularity and digital marking; logical completeness; the ability to formulate the concept of the text by linguistic means (Shulekina, 2018).

Problem Statement

Today the concept visual literacy is becoming more popular and therefore, a large number of empirical studies in education and psychology are devoted to the study of visualization (Fu et al., 2019; McVicker, 2018; Richter et al., 2016). Infographics are actively used as a new space for reading, learning and developing mental skills (Parkinson, 2016; Wang et al., 2019; Yildirim, 2016). So, polycode texts are actively integrated into modern education as a mean of improving the quality of children’s education. For example, in English-speaking countries nowadays public policy stimulates the education system to use polycode texts as a tool to support students in the study space. The corpus of special visual texts is a part of the general basic state standards, especially for teaching the native (English) language (Bakis, 2012; Edwards, 2019).

Many authors see the connection between the ability to read comics and reading a language text. Despite the fact that some teachers still pay little attention to comics as a didactic resource, the attitude to comics is changing in actual educational space. Today most researchers emphasize the importance of skills to understand comics for the development of educational literacy, including the mastery of reading and writing (Afrilyasanti & Basthomi, 2011; Barbee, 2015; Bowkett, 2011; Briggs, 2015; Dallacqua, 2012; Drolet, 2010). All of these authors show the comics’ advantages in learning students reading and writing skills, which are the part of different types of literacy.

Comics are very important tool for the development of children's thinking processes and text comprehension. On the basis of visual literacy, it is possible to develop metacognitive thinking skills (Kholodnaja & Gelfman, 2016; Marshall, 2018; Mendez Leon & Cardenas, 2018). Research confirms that the developed skills of understanding visual material are transferred to the skills of understanding the language text in traditional reading.

Research Questions

Evaluation of understanding processes in children of primary school age on the basis of visual narratives, which include comics, is significant for studying the educational opportunities of different students in the cognitive processing of complex polycode information in a comprehensive (including inclusive) school. While reading graphic texts, children do not passively perceive information but interact both with text and pictures to construct meaning. Moreover, a great didactic potential is detected in the application of polycode texts while working with children who have special educational needs (Shulekina, 2018).

The list of research questions includes the following:

  • Is there a difference in understanding traditional language and visual narrative texts?

  • What types of texts are most preferable for primary school children in the study group as a whole?

  • How do girls and boys understand different types of texts separately?

Purpose of the Study

In this regard our multidisciplinary team of researchers has chosen the study target. We intended to test the hypothesis about the various mechanisms of understanding polycode and traditional language narrative texts by children of primary school age, as well as to identify the gender characteristics of using these text types using empirical material.

Research Methods

The assessment of primary school students’ thinking skills in various types of narratives during the research was carried out on the basis of a sample of second-grade students of Moscow mainstream schools who do not have deviations in intellectual and speech development. The sample was formed on the basis of a deterministic approach, based on the simplicity of establishing contacts with students in order to obtain information necessary for conducting the research. The sample included 94 students – 52 girls (55.3%) and 42 boys (44.7%). The structure of the obtained sample makes it possible to objectively evaluate gender differences in the development of skills while using narrative materials in elementary school.

The experiment was organized in accordance with the technology by Ju. Shulekina (2018) and adapted to the objectives of this study. All the participants of the survey were offered various types of tasks with narrative texts – to read, answer the questions on the text and retell. The texts were presented both in the traditional linguistic form (“As a hedgehog was walking in the woods” by G. Tsyferov; “About Stepan and sour cream”, a children's counting rhyme), as well as the polycode form (comics: 1) “Pifa’s breakfast” and 2) “Pifa’s fishing”, 3) comic book “Wind”). The polycode stimulus material was chosen so that the order of frames in each comic was different: the comic book “Pifa’s breakfast” had a vertical arrangement of frames, the comic book “Pifa’s fishing” had a horizontal arrangement of frames, the comic book “Wind” had a vector structure assuming a zigzag perception of the order of the main hero’s actions. Images of comic texts are shown in figure 01 .

Figure 1: Polycode texts – stimulus study material
Polycode texts – stimulus study material
See Full Size >

The ability of second-grade students to work with narrative texts of various types was evaluated according to the following parameters:

• Understanding the external plan of the text associated with the analysis of the linear order of sentences organized according to linguistic rules and reflecting the semantic content of the text.

• Understanding the internal plan of the text. It means understanding the semantic organization of the material, including the hidden meaning.

• Modeling of one’s own history, that is, retelling the text in your own words following the traces of looking or reading the text.

• The need for help when working with a text – the need to obtain various types of verbal support from the experimenter (suggestive and specific questions, introductory words, gestures initiating speech activity, etc.) and the effectiveness of their usage by students.

Each parameter had an assessment scale: total task completion (25%), partial task completion (12.5%) and task failure (0%). The overall success of the participants in the experiment was the sum of the points scored for each of four parameters. Based on these criteria, the estimated range of a particular pupil can vary from 0% (complete lack of skills) to 100% (maximum level of skills development while working with narrative material).

The following final scale for assessing the success of studied students in each type of narrative material and during the whole experiment is proposed to use by us (table 1 ).

Table 1 -
See Full Size >

The study assessed the success of working with narrative material both as a whole in the sample and according to gender structure. The final assessment was carried out by averaging the estimated values of each student according to:

  • types of studied narrative materials;

  • evaluated parameters in accordance with the methodology;

  • gender structure of the sample.

Averaging was carried out using the arithmetic mean of average (formula 1). A similar method was used in the study of the authors (Shulekina & Dmitrieva, 2019).

x ̄ = x i n , (1)

where n - the number of the studied contingent of primary school students during the experiment;

xi - the obtained estimated values for each studied parameter.

Findings

The results of assessing the success of students in the context of individual types of narrative materials and in the whole experiment are shown in table 02 .

Table 2 -
See Full Size >

The results which are clearly presented in table 2 are shown in figure 02 .

So, the data in table 2 indicate that the average percentage of success of all types of work according to the sample of studied pupils was shown at the level of 59.3%, which corresponds to a high level of success of children. Particular attention should be paid to the absence of significant gender differences in the success of mastering narrative material by studied pupils (58.9% girls’ success and 59.6% boys’ success).

When assessing success for certain types of work, it should be noted that the highest level of success was demonstrated by students working with vector comics (67.9%) and poetic texts (65.5%). It is necessary to pay attention to the significant gender difference according to these types of narratives: while working with vector comics, the average success rate for girls was 77.3%, while the average success rate for boys was 57.1%. Vice versa, when working with poetic texts, the boys showed an average success rate of 75.0%, while the average success rate for girls was 56.0%. The evaluation of the success while working with horizontal comics and a prosaic text during the experiment did not reveal significant gender differences.

Figure 2: The total average level of success while working with various types of narratives among primary school students
The total average level of success while working with various types of narratives among
      primary school students
See Full Size >

We also consider, it is necessary to pay attention to the general heterogeneity of the plurality in terms of the parameter for evaluating the success of work with all types of narrative texts. According to this parameter the coefficient of variation is 43.22% (which is significantly higher than 33%, which is a measure of the homogeneity of the plurality) and the dispersion of the success level for separate pupils ranges from 12.5% to 100%, which is typical for elementary school classes in ordinary comprehensive schools in Moscow.

The coefficient of variation was determined on the basis of the standard methodology for its calculation – by the ratio of the standard deviation to the average value of the success rating for all types of narratives (formula 2) [2]:

V σ = σ x - (2),

where x - is the average success for all types of narratives;

σ   is standard deviation, which is calculated by the formula (formula 3):

σ = x i - x ̄ 2 n (3)

Further, during the processing of experimental data, we estimated the average level of success of the pupils for each parameter of the assessment as a whole for the sample and taking into account gender differences (tables 3 5 ).

Table 3 -
See Full Size >
Table 4 -
See Full Size >
Table 5 -
See Full Size >

The data in tables 3 5 indicate that the greatest difficulty for students was caused by an understanding of the essential basis of narrative material. The success of understanding the internal plan of the text is minimal among all the estimated parameters both in the sample as a whole and in terms of the gender structure of the sample: at the maximum possible value of 25%, the average value of this indicator in the sample as a whole was 13.6%, for girls – 12. 8%, for boys – 14.5%. The maximum level of success was demonstrated by the pupils in terms of formal perception of the content of the narrative. In terms of the understanding of the external plan of the text, the success rate was 17.9% in the sample as a whole, 18.0% for girls and 17.8% for boys. An analysis of retelling and use of verbal support when working with narratives in elementary school students did not reveal significant gender differences.

Very interesting results were obtained during a consolidated assessment of the success of working with polycode and text (monocode) narratives, the results of which are presented in table 6 for each parameter being evaluated.

Table 6 -
See Full Size >

The results which are presented in table 6 demonstrate significant superiority in the essential aspect of understanding the content of text narratives compared to polycode texts. So, at the highest possible level of success of 25%, the average estimated value of the skills of drawing up the internal plan of the text is 18.2% for text narratives and for polycode texts, the same estimated parameter is successful only for 10.5%, which is a very significant difference.

The explicit gender differences while working with different types of narratives should be noted: girls demonstrate greater success in mastering skills while working with polycode narratives (table 6 , comparison of data in columns 3 and 4 for all assessment parameters). In our opinion, the obtained results are a consequence of the greater significance of the visual component of the object-spatial environment for women.

Boys, on the contrary, are significantly ahead of girls in terms of working with textual narratives in all evaluated parameters, as it is shown by columns 6 and 7 of table 6 .

According to the authors, such results may be the result of males’ accelerated development of abstract thinking compared with females already at the stage of education in elementary school.

Graphically, the results of an average assessment of the success of students with polycode and text narratives are presented in figures 03 - 04 .

Figure 3: Average assessment of the success of elementary school students with polycode narratives
Average assessment of the success of elementary school students with polycode narratives
See Full Size >
Figure 4: Average assessment of the success of elementary school students with text (monocode) narratives
Average assessment of the success of elementary school students with text (monocode)
      narratives
See Full Size >

Conclusion

According to the current stage of the study the following preliminary conclusions were suggested:

  • Gender differences of the second-grade students in the formation of skills while working with various types of narratives were determined: boys demonstrate advancing skills in working with textual (monocode) narratives, and girls demonstrate with polycode.

Among the narratives presented in various modalities, visual narratives (comic book) and poetic narratives are the best for second-grade students’ understanding. At the same time, the order of frames in the visual narrative (comic) is important for a better understanding of the content and the construction of meaning. Comics with vector frames turned out to be the most understandable for children of the studied age in comparison with comics with horizontal and vertical frames. The conclusion is confirmed by the obtained results of the experiment with second-grade students of primary schools in Moscow: with an average value of the success rate of working with various types of narratives at the level of 59.3%, students demonstrated success in working with vector comics at the level of 67.9 %, and with poetic texts – at the level of 65.5% with the highest possible assessment of the success of working with narrative materials in the amount of 100%.

During the research it was determined that students have difficulties in understanding the essential basis of narrative materials: the percentage of success in understanding the internal plan of the narrative was 13.6%, with the maximum possible grade for this parameter being 25.0%. The best results were demonstrated by students in terms of formal perception of narratives: with an understanding of the external plan, the average success rate was 17.9% with the maximum possible estimate of the parameter at 25%.

In terms of understanding the essential basis of narrative materials, a significant superiority was also found in the perception of the content of text narratives compared to polycode ones: with the highest possible success rate of 25%, the average estimated value for understanding the internal plan of the text is 18.2% for text narratives, and for polycodes – only 10.5%, which is a very significant difference. In this regard, we consider it is necessary to continue research on this issue.

References

  1. Afrilyasanti, R., & Basthomi, Y. (2011). Adapting Comics and Cartoons to Develop 21st Century Learners. Language in India, 11(11), 552-568.
  2. Bakis, M. (2012). The graphic novel classroom: Powerful teaching and learning with images. Thousand Oaks. Corwin Press.
  3. Barbee, M. (2015). Comic Books as Models for Literacy Instruction. International Reading Association. Retrieved from https://www.literacyworldwide.org/blog/literacy-now/2015/08/12/comic-books-as-models-for-literacy-instruction.
  4. Bowkett, S. (2011). Using comic art to improve speaking, reading and writing. Routledge, Taylor & Francis.
  5. Brenna, B. (2012). How Graphic Novels Support Reading Comprehension Strategy Development in Children. Literacy Journal. UKLA, 47, 88-94.
  6. Briggs, S. (2015). Reading with Pictures: Serious Learning Through Comics. https://www.opencolleges.edu.au/informed/features/reading-with-pictures-serious-learning-through-comics/
  7. Cohn, N. (2013). The visual language of comics: Introduction to the structure and cognition of sequential images. Bloomsbury.
  8. Dallacqua, A. K. (2012). Exploring Literary Devices in Graphic Novels. Language Arts, 9(6), 365-378.
  9. Drolet, C. A. (2010). Using Comics in the Development of EFL Reading and Writing. SungKyul University.
  10. Dunst, A., Laubrock, J., & Wildfeuer, J. (2018). Empirical Comics Research: Digital, Multimodal, and Cognitive Methods. Routledge.
  11. Edwards, K. D. (2019). Texts as Tools: Engaging Students in Reading to Complement Hands-On Activities. https://www.questia.com/read/1G1-568147867/texts-as-tools-engaging-students-in-reading-to-complement.
  12. Fu, X., Wang, Y., Dong, H., Cui, W., & Zhang, H. (2019). Visualization Assessment: A Machine Learning Approach. IEEE Visualization Conference (VIS), 126-130.
  13. Kholodnaja, M. A., & Gelfman, E. G. (2016). Developing educational texts as a tool of intellectual education of students. RAN Institute of Psychology Press.
  14. Marshall, J. (2018). The Power of Comics. https://literacyworldwide.org/blog%2Fliteracy-daily%2F2018%2F12%2F20%2Fthe-power-of-comics
  15. Martín-Arnala, L. A., Leóna, J. A., Broekb, P., & Olmosa, R. (2019). Understanding Comics. A Comparison between Children and Adults through a Coherence. Incoherence Paradigm in an Eye-tracking Study. Psicología Educativa, 25(2), 127-137.
  16. McVicker, C. J. (2018). Visual Literacy And Learning To Read: Using Comic Strips for Reading Instruction. Journal of Visual Languages & Computing. No. 8. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/327043053_Visual_Literacy_And_Learning_To_Read_Using_Comic_Strips_for_Reading_Instruction
  17. Mendez Leon, A. P., & Cardenas, B. A. (2018). Improving reading comprehension through comics as a narrative text. Monography. Сorporación universitaria minuto de dios education faculty.
  18. Parkinson, M. (2016). Infographic tips and tools. Talent Development, 70(5), 26-28.
  19. Richter, J., Scheiter, K., & Eitel, A. (2016). Signaling text-picture relations in multimedia learning: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 17, 19-36.
  20. Schnotz, W. (2014). An Integrated Model of Text and Picture Comprehension. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning (2nd edition, pp. 72-103). Cambridge University Press.
  21. Shulekina, J. A. (2018). Modern technologies for working with a textbook in inclusive education. MCU Press.
  22. Shulekina, J. A., & Dmitrieva, O. V. (2019). The Formation Working Skills with a textbook in primary school children. Vestnik MCU, 4, 83-92.
  23. Wang, Z., Wang, S., Farinella, M., Murray-Rust, D., Henry Riche, N., & Bach, B. (2019). Comparing Effectiveness and Engagement of Data Comics and Infographics. CHI '19 Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Glasgow, Scotland Uk, 253. https://doi.org/10.1145/3290605.3300483
  24. Yildirim, S. (2016). Infographics for educational purposes: Their structure, properties, and reader approaches. Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 15(3), 98-110.

Copyright information

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

About this article

Publication Date

20 November 2020

eBook ISBN

978-1-80296-094-5

Publisher

European Publisher

Volume

95

Print ISBN (optional)

-

Edition Number

1st Edition

Pages

1-1241

Subjects

Sociolinguistics, discourse analysis, bilingualism, multilingualism

Cite this article as:

Shulekina, J. A., Dmitrieva, O. V., Borodina, M. A., & Kireeva, I. P. (2020). Narratives Of Which Modality Are The Most Accessible For Younger Students’ Understanding?. In Е. Tareva, & T. N. Bokova (Eds.), Dialogue of Cultures - Culture of Dialogue: from Conflicting to Understanding, vol 95. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 1175-1185). European Publisher. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2020.11.03.124