Abstract
Mental training is an important component of general training, which contributes to the increase of individual performance and perfecting the execution of motor acts. Its purpose is to form representations with the help of motor intelligence, through which the processed movement, which resembles its design, can be adapted to concrete athletic tasks. Aside from the fact that the adaptation occurs in record time, it also helps find alternatives to the motor situations, which are strategic in an athletic environment, based on motor schemes and ideomotor representations updated in a short time and in agreement with the action. The investigative research that we have initiated aims to highlight the influence of mental training on the athletic performance and identify a formula for mental training that can be applied in the times when the athlete loses focus. The ideomotor technique applied to the subjects was innovative, the results of the research supporting the idea of performance achieved through this type of training, as an important part of the athletes’ evolution. After applying the mental training program, the explosive force of the subjects of our research has increased and, moreover, the results can validate the technique of applying this type of intervention in favour of athletic performance.
Keywords: Mental trainingvisualisation(self-) motivationjumpsheart rate
Introduction
According to Markova (2004), in order to create correct mental representations, it is of help if we use the appropriate words – otherwise it will harm the understanding, and the intensity of the image can help or hinder knowledge. At the same time as the language, the intensity of the mental image can facilitate or hinder knowledge and the learning processes. Mental representations can be true or false, they are “mirrors or signs of nature” that generate formalisation or symbolic structures that have an integrative and socialising role. The vast polysemy of the term ‘representation’ can direct us to a multitude of phenomena that make it a concept that is hard to identify socially, but which refers to individual, inter-individual, inter-group and ideological processes that interact and provide the larger dynamics that defines reality and makes it accessible to the activity within the microcosm of sports and motor skills. Representations of all kinds are “the product of our actions and communications” (Moscovici, 1984, p. 12), the image that stands at the border between the hostile reality and ourselves. They can be a system of values, notions and practices related to objects, aspects or dimensions of the environment in which we work that allow us to understand the material space and the adjustment of behaviour according to any new variable (Abalașei, 2009). Depending on the context, the representation through the use of motor intelligence is what adapts movement to the situation at hand. Aside from the fact that the adjustment is accomplished in record time, new solutions, strategies or alternatives to the situation in the athletic environment are found – based on the motor schemes and the ideomotor representations that are actualised very fast. According to Horghidan (2000, p. 104), motor intelligence is expressed and works through specific finalisation. The creation of mental images or mental representations is achieved through the implication of each individual sense, as well as all of them combined. The kinetic sense, for example, is the one responsible for the position of the body in space, the presence or the movement that is born through the stimulation of sensitive nerve endings of the muscles, tendons and joints. Representations in the field of physical education and sports are formed by acquiring physical and psychological techniques that lead to the construction of an ideal image of the process to be executed. The nervous system possesses a complex servomechanism resembling a system for automatic chasing of a target, which guides itself towards that target through feedback and stored information. In his opinion, this servomechanism can be programmed to visualise the objective by using mental imagery, steering itself towards reaching the target with a higher efficiency than if using conscious effort or willpower. The brain will process the imagined information as though it came from real experiences, and therefore this will have a similar effect on the brain as real experiences. So, the psychological-neural-muscular theory, stated by Mikicin (2016), says that an event that is imagined with high intensity can trigger neural and muscular reactions that are similar to those of the real experience, due to a series of correlations between the state of attention, of preparation of a response to a certain stimulus (conscious attention), the state of response with heightened attention (automated attention, unconscious attention) and the alpha state. The images produced in the brain transmit impulses to the muscles with the purpose of executing the imagined movements, although the impulses can be so weak as to not produce movement. Some articles in the literature speak of forming neural paths that create a plan, a map that later helps the subject to execute the movement in question. It is this map relying on the cognitive component that forms the basis of sports performance (Pesce et al., 2016).
According to Buck, Hutchinson, Winter and Thompson (2016), the mental motor schema formed with the help of mental training can raise the efficacy of the execution – after they analysed self-efficacy of two groups participating in a research focused on the improvement of athletic performance through this alternative method.
Problem Statement
Sports training is increasingly complex. This leads to the athlete’s mental approach, often this component making the difference between the winner and the loser. For this reason, we want to study aspects related to the mental component of sports training.
Research Questions
The hypothesis of the paper resides in identifying if:
mental training influences athletic performance;
we can find a formula of mental training that can be applied in the break of a game or at any time when the athlete loses focus.
Purpose of the Study
We wish to prove that, if mental training is performed correctly, it can be later used as a means for the (self-) motivation of the athletes, for example: it can be used prior to the start of the competitions, but also in the breaks between sets or rounds.
Research Methods
The subjects consisted of a group of 40 students from the Physical Education and Sports Faculty, divided into 2 subgroups of 20 people each. Of the 40 subjects, 13 were female and the rest of 27 were male. The subjects were classified into 2 groups: group A included 6 female and 7 male students who were professional athletes, and the rest, up to 20 people, was made up of students who were not professional athletes. Group B consisted only of students who were not involved in performance sports: 7 females and 13 males. Practicing performance sports was the sole criteria for directing the subjects into one of the two groups. The two groups, A and B, had to follow slightly different protocols. The protocol for Group A was the following: the subjects underwent the initial testing – each of them performed three jumps of each of the following types: squat jump, counter movement jump and free jump (details below); we then applied the POLAR Pro Team device (details below); a 30-minute mental training session (details below); second testing for all three jumps (executed in the same order as the previous time); a 15-minute mental training recall session; third (final) testing: the subjects executed the jumps while being reminded the key-words that they had identified during the mental training session. The protocol applied to Group B was: the subjects underwent the initial testing – each of them performed three jumps: squat jump, counter movement jump and free jump (details below); we then applied the POLAR Pro Team device (details below); a 30-minute mental training session (details below); second testing for all three jumps (executed in the same order as the previous time).
We differentiated the sessions, because our intention was: to observe whether one session of mental training could significantly increase the amplitude of the jumps; to observe whether there were improvements in the amplitude of the jumps when two sessions of mental training were applied. A brief discussion of the methods used for testing: The three types of jumps that we used were: Squat Jump (SJ): the test focuses on evaluating the explosive power of the lower limbs. The recording of the jump (its height) is done through the use of Just Jump; Counter Movement Jump (CMJ): the test focuses on evaluating the elastic explosive power of the lower limbs. SJ measures the concentric contraction power of the extensors of the knees. The particularity of this jump consists in the starting position: standing with the knees bent at 90°, and this must be maintained for one second – which implies an isolate isometric contraction, unlike the CMJ, which measures the contracting power of the same muscle groups – but in this case, it is an eccentric contraction that is immediately followed by a concentric one, which leads to a higher power due to the elastic energy of the muscles; Free Jump (FJ): the test focuses on evaluating the elastic explosive power in the legs.
The words used were not special in any way, their sole purpose was to connect the conscious mind to the unlimited potential of the subconscious mind that is accessed in a state of relaxation. The words, once heard, become triggers for the individual performance.
Findings
As we mentioned in the description of the methods, the difference between the two groups – A and B – resides in the fact that the subjects from group A had one extra 15-minute session of mental training. Given the multitude of values and in order to make the study data easier to follow, we analysed, for each subject, the difference between the values obtained for the jumps, i.e.: the value of the second jump – the initial value; therefore, in the tables included in the work, we have SJ2-SJ1, SJ3-SJ2, CMJ2-CMJ1, CMJ3-CMJ1 and FJ2-FJ1, FJ3-FJ1 (Table
The absolute values have individual significance; from the point of view of the study, we were more interested in seeing whether there were any differences between the jumps, how many of the subjects showed an increase in the number of centimetres following a mental training session and whether repeating the session would bring an increase in the value of the jumps.
The data showed better values for group B, although group A was the one who included the professional athletes, and their initial values were slightly superior, as can be noticed in Table
The situation is somewhat unexpected, but the explanation that we have identified relates to the fact that the subjects who practice professional sports expect the improvement of their results to depend solely on the training, and are not used to rely on mental training. The subjects in group B had no particular expectations, they were curious regarding what was happening and approached the testing as a game – all these being premises for the learning and evolution process (Bănică, 2009; Drosescu, 2009). The values obtained by subjects in groups A and B for the initial test and the second test, for all three jumps, are shown in Table
For the SJ jump, of the 40 subjects included in the study, 23 (57.5%) obtained positive results in the second testing, 2 (5%) did not see any change, while 15 (37.5%) achieved a less good result than in the first testing. For the CMJ jump, of the 40 subjects included in the study, 20 (50%) obtained positive results in the second testing, 3 (7.5%) did not see any change, while 17 (42.5%) achieved a less good result than in the first testing. The FJ jump was executed with higher values in 20 (40%) cases, 4 subjects (10%) did not see any modification, while 20 (50%) achieved a less good result than in the first testing. From the above data, we can observe firstly that, in the cases of SJ and CMJ jumps, the mental training session has added centimetres to the initial value. For FJ, the increase in the value of the jump was smaller than that for SJ and CMJ. Also from Table
The data that we present are obtained after only 30 minutes of mental training and a 15-minute recall session – of the total 45 minutes, the visualisation itself lasting only about 16-17 minutes; another aspect brought forward by the data in the table relates to the competitive spirit and the manner in which the subjects have responded to the challenge of this study: columns 7 through 12 show the average (in centimetres) for the difference between the value of the third jump and the value of the second jump: 1.24, 1.67 and 0.87 cm for males and 0.72, 0.13 and 0.21 cm for females. We interpret this increase in centimetres in the third testing, compared to the second one, for each of the three jumps (SJ, CMJ, FJ), as being the effect of self-motivation once they saw that after the first session of mental training they have achieved progress, which has led to better participation and more intense work in the 15-minute session. The statement is reflected in the number of subjects who have obtained improved results in the third jump: 11 subjects for SJ, 13 for CMJ and 8 for FJ; and in females, 5 for SJ, 3 for CMJ and 4 for FJ. We wish to underline these data as the numbers of 11 and 13 subjects are calculated out of a total of 14 males, and the numbers 5 and 4 are out of a total of 6 females.
When we analysed the results, we noticed another aspect useful to our study: the percentage of those who achieved improved results in all jumps was 35%. One of the females included in the study obtained negative results in both the second and the third jump due to muscle soreness after the training session she had had prior to our study.
Throughout the mental training sessions, we monitored the cardiac frequency with the help of the POLAR Pro Team device. Statistical calculations in the GraphPad Prism 6 software led to the conclusion that there were statistically significant differences, as can be seen in Figure
A-FC REP (beats/minute) – the value of the average and the standard deviation for the cardiac frequency at rest for group A; A-FC_30’ (beats/minute) – the value of the average and the standard deviation for the cardiac frequency at rest for group A throughout the 30’ mental training; A-FC_15’ (beats/minute) – the value of the average and the standard deviation for the cardiac frequency at rest for group A throughout the 15’ mental training; B-FC_REP (beats/minute) – the value of the average and the standard deviation for the cardiac frequency at rest for group B; B-FC_30’ (beats/minute) – the value of the average and the standard deviation for the cardiac frequency at rest for group B throughout the 30’ mental training.
The difference between the data series can be evaluated with the help of p, whose values can range as follows: p ≤ 0.05(*), p ≤ 0.01(**), p ≤ 0.001(***) or p ≤ 0.0001(****). The increase in the number of stars expresses a higher, more statistically significant difference.
Conclusion
The interpretation of the data from Figure
References
- Abalașei, B. A. (2009). Reprezentarea socială a violenței în sport. Iași: Editura Universității “Alexandru Ioan Cuza”.
- Bănică, L. (2009). Psihomotricitate, psihopatologie, psihoterapie. Elemente aplicative în kinetoterapie. Bucureşti: Editura Fundaţiei România de Mâine.
- Buck, D. J. M., Hutchinson, J. C., Winter, C. R., & Thompson, B. A. (2016). The effects of mental imagery with video-modeling on self-efficacy and maximal front squat ability. Sports, 4(2): 23. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3390/sports4020023
- Drosescu, P. (2009). Visualisation techniques in athlete’s early recovery after trauma and training. Journal of Sport Science & Medicine, 8(11): 18. Retrieved from https://www.jssm.org/suppls/11/invitedspeakers.pdf
- Franz, A. (2008). Medicina psihosomatică. București: Editura Trei.
- Horghidan, V. (2000). Problematica psihomotricității. București: Editura Globus.
- Markova, I. (2004). Dialogistica şi reprezentările sociale. Iaşi: Editura Polirom.
- Mikicin, M. (2016). State of mind as a subjective mental sensation results from objective brain activity following neurofeedback-EEG and relaxation trainings. Acta Neuropsychologica, 14(1), 17-33.
- Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. Social representations (pp. 3-69). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Pesce, C., Marchetti, R., Forte, R., Crova, C., Scatigna, M., Goudas, M., & Danish, S. J. (2016). Youth life skills training: Exploring outcomes and mediating mechanisms of a group-randomized trial in physical education. Sport Exercise and Performance Psychology, 5(3), 232-246.
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
16 February 2019
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-054-9
Publisher
Future Academy
Volume
55
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-752
Subjects
Sports, sport science, physical education
Cite this article as:
Drosescu, P., Abalașei, B., Honceriu, C., & Trofin, F. P. (2019). The Importance Of Mental Training For The Evolution Of Individual Performance. In V. Grigore, M. Stănescu, M. Stoicescu, & L. Popescu (Eds.), Education and Sports Science in the 21st Century, vol 55. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 744-752). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2019.02.92