Abstract
Background. In order to deepen understanding of signal detection/discrimination processes we have to focus on highlighting individual differences in observers’ sensory performance due to the contribution of various variables of personality and cognition spheres. The purpose of our study was to test cognitive style factors (augmenting-reducing, levelling-sharpening, flexibility-rigidity of cognitive control, equivalence range, and focusing-scanning) influencing performance of psychophysical tasks. Methods. Ninety participants performed a set of cognitive style tests as well as two psychophysical tasks on visual signal detection (‘yes-no’) and loudness discrimination (‘same-different’). The duration of visual pattern presentation and difference between pairs of auditory stimuli were used to provide task’s difficulty level, and therefore the level of uncertainty. Results. Data analysis showed several effects of cognitive styles on psychophysical tasks performance indices, in particular: sensory sensitivity, RT and its stability, and response confidence. According to our results, each style is related to its own benefits and advantages in observer’s overall productivity. Furthermore, the contribution of cognitive styles differed depending on task’s type and difficulty level. Conclusions. Our results support current findings, considering cognitive styles as playing a regulative role in cognitive activity. Hence, they could be acknowledged as tools, mediating individual strategies, representing different ways of coping with perceptual uncertainty.
Keywords: Cognitive stylespsychophysicsindividual differencessignal detectionsignal discrimination
Introduction
Multiple studies showed the effect of subject or personality factors on observer’s sensory performance, which nonetheless were rarely taken into consideration. However, it was later acknowledged that contribution of stimulation conditions, or ‘stimulus factors’, provides only partial explanation of perceiver’s behaviour, emphasizing the necessity to consider subject factors as well (Gurler et al., 2015; Siegel, Kelly, 2017; Wilson et al., 2016).
We believe that threshold task performance is associated with a conflict between the need to solve the task successfully and observer’s available resources. It has been widely studied in line with resource approach, suggesting that cognitive processes represent the system with limited amount of available cognitive resources. Resources, for their part, are considered as a pool of energy, flexibly distributed during information processing (Epling et al., 2016; Humphreys, Revelle, 1984; Parasuraman et al., 1987; Ralph et al., 2017; Smit et al., 2004; Thomson et al., 2015). Demands and conditions of psychophysical tasks serve as a basis for engaging the methodology of resource approach since they impose several restrictions to observer’s available resources.
The stated conflict may be solved in different ways, reflected in the choice of various tools and strategies, regulated, in its turn, by high-level mechanisms of observer’s mental activity. We suggest cognitive styles (CS) as one of such regulative mechanisms.
The concept of CS was initially introduced as a result of considering a qualitatively different aspect of human cognitive sphere, referred to individual differences in a way or manner of cognitive functioning, i.e. organizing, representing and processing information (Kozhevnikov et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2012). In accordance with this approach, same results of solving various types of tasks can be achieved in different ways, such as ways of perceiving and understanding the task, as well as various characteristics of tempo, efficiency and mistakes made (Nosal, 2009). Hence, this is an important factor to examine since CS could provide an explanation of individual variabilities in threshold tasks performance.
Problem Statement
There is a large body of research that has been conducted into the role of stimulus factors in observer’s performance indices and strategies used, whereas the contribution and weight of individual differences factors is still underestimated in psychophysics. While the final performance can be partially explained by stimulus variables, the study of individual differences is an issue of critical importance.
We would like to highlight the crucial point that psychophysical tasks have to be considered as related to high workload and high perceptual uncertainty. As a result, such task specifities and demands force observer to look for any appropriate compensation strategies, which allow overcoming the distinct deficit of sensory information.
The present tasks cause resource costs due to (1) random and rapid presentation of low-intensive stimulation, (2) monotonous and long-term procedure, (3) successive character of stimuli presentation, (4) spatial uncertainty in localisation of target stimulus. In addition to mentioned above, these vigilance tasks call for engagement of additional effort, reflected in short-term memory and attention allocation resources. Furthermore, observer has to maintain a high vigilance level and sustain high tempo over prolonged period of performing the task.
Research Questions
In this study we have endeavoured to address two main issues, referring to the wide psychological context of individual differences role in cognitive tasks solving. First, we were wondering whether differences in sensory performance exist between subjects representing certain CS, and hence, whether CS studies are fruitful avenue in differential psychophysics. The second addressed issue was the following: does the contribution of CS factors change due to variation of task type and level of perceptual uncertainty?
Purpose of the Study
The purpose of our study was to test CS factors (augmenting-reducing, levelling-sharpening, flexibility-rigidity of cognitive control, equivalence range, and focusing-scanning) influencing sensory performance indices under different task type (visual signal detection –
Research Methods
Participants
A total of 90 participants (28 males and 62 females) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision took part in this experiment.
Software & Apparatus
The experiment was run on IBM-compatible PC with a clean Windows XP Professional 32 bit operating system, in which all background processes were turned off. The stimuli were presented on a 22” LCD monitor, with a resolution of 1920 × 1080. Participants viewed the monitor from a distance of 60 cm.
Since our tasks suggest short duration of stimulus presentation, the latter was administered through retrace control procedure. RT was registered using a special USB response pad, providing the precision of ± 5 ms.
All experiment tasks were created using ‘Practice MSU’ integrated computer system (UMK Psychology Company, Russia, http://psychosoft.ru).
Stimuli
In
The duration of visual pattern presentation (90 or 60 ms) and difference between pairs of auditory stimuli (2 or 1 dB) were used to provide task’s difficulty level, and therefore the level of uncertainty. Each task consisted of five series: (1) introductory – 10 trials, (2) easy training – 30 trials, (3) hard training – 30 trials, (4) main easy – 100 trials, and (5) main hard – 100 trials.
Procedure
Participants started experimental session with performing two psychophysical tasks. In
Sensory sensitivity (Aˈ), strictness of criterion index (YesRate), RT, RT stability (SDRT) and confidence (Conf) were assessed for each task.
After psychophysical tasks participants performed a set of CS tests:
Leveling-Sharpening House Test (Santostefano, 1971);
Stroop Color-Word Interference Test (Stroop, 1935), assessing flexibility-rigidity of cognitive control;
Object Sorting Test (Gardner et al., 1959), evaluating equivalence range;
Size Estimation Test (Gardner et al., 1959), appraising focusing-scanning and augmenting-reducing.
Findings
One-way ANOVAs showed several significant effects of CS on psychophysical tasks performance indices. The Tables
Augmenting-reducing
Augmenters showed higher accuracy and speed in hard tasks due to their underlying ability to perform under conditions of extremely low stimulation intensity (Larsen, Zarate, 1991). The reducers’ strategy to underestimate perceived intensity of incoming stimulation has proved to be less efficient in the hard
Leveling-sharpening
Sharpeners showed advantages in both accuracy and speed of solving vigilance tasks. We suggest that the possible explanation of found differences between levelers and sharpeners could be provided by individual features of perceiving and remembering the incoming sensory information as well as comparing it to the previous sensory impressions (Gardner et al., 1959; Santostefano, 1971). Since the stimuli in both our tasks were presented successively, not simultaneously, such task conditions impose an extra workload on short-term memory resources (Humphreys, Revelle, 1984; Parasuraman et al., 1987). Thus, sharpeners tend to shape and create more detailed and precise image of perceived sensory stimulation, which have allowed them to solve the tasks quickly and accurately at the same time.
Flexibility-rigidity of cognitive control
The results indicated that ‘flexible’ subjects showed higher sensory sensitivity in
Regarding dynamic aspects of task performance, the advantage in RT was shown by subjects with ‘rigidity of cognitive control’. We believe that found differences reflect the depth of information processing. ‘Rigid’ subjects therefore spend less time on response, in contrast to ‘flexible’ ones, who deal with sensory information in depth. Moreover, the features described above could serve as a possible explanation of quasi-significant differences in confidence level. Since ‘rigid’ subjects do not tend to analyse the incoming sensory information in depth, the threshold of evaluating their answer as confident is lower as it is based on fewer stimuli characteristics.
Equivalence range
Contrary to our initial assumptions, no significant differences in sensory sensitivity were found for this CS. It seems that the tendency to build upon the differences between objects refers to person’s conceptual sphere, i.e. the number of categories in individual mental experience and specifities of intellectual activity in general, rather than in perceptive sphere.
However, the significant differences in confidence level were found for equivalence range. We believe that it is due to ‘narrow’ subjects’ strategy of drawing the attention to differences between stimuli and a more differentiated categorization of sensory experience (Gardner et al., 1959).
Focusing-scanning
We suggest that scanners showed advantage in sensory sensitivity in both
In contrast, focusers showed higher confidence in hard
Conclusion
To conclude, our approach allows considering both subject’s activity (CS) and stimulus factors (task type and uncertainty level), determining observer’s performance.
The results are crucial in the light of the controversial issue of style value (Zhang et al., 2012). We believe that each style is related to its own benefits and advantages — for instance, either accuracy or speed of performing a task. Neither of style groups should be considered as more or less effective in general; they rather may or may not correspond to the certain conditions and demands in solving the particular cognitive task. However, we stress once again that the same final efficiency could be associated with different resource costs, and furthermore could be achieved in different ways or manners.
We would like to highlight the crucial point that CS should be considered as tools, mediating individual strategies representing different ways of coping with perceptual uncertainty. This understanding is supported by up-to-date findings concerning the role of CS in cognition regulation (Kozhevnikov et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2012).
Acknowledgments
This study was supported in part by the Lomonosov Moscow State University Program of Development. We acknowledge A. Kremlev for the development of computer tests we used.
References
- Epling, S.L., Russell, P.N., & Helton, W.S. (2016). A new semantic vigilance task: vigilance decrement, workload, and sensitivity to dual-task costs. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 133–139. doi:
- Gardner, R. W., Holzman, P. S., Klein, G. S., Linton, H. B., & Spence, D. P. (1959). Cognitive control. A study of individual consistencies in cognitive behavior: Part 4. Psychological issues. New York: International Universities Press.
- Gurler, D., Doyle, N., Walker, E., Magnotti, J., & Beauchamp, M. (2015). A link between individual differences in multisensory speech perception and eye movements. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 77(4), 1333–1341. doi:
- Humphreys, M.S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation and performance: a theory of relationship between individual differences and information processing. Psychological Review, 91, 153–184.
- Kozhevnikov, M., Evans, C., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2014). Cognitive style as environmentally sensitive individual differences in cognition: A modern synthesis and applications in education, business, and management. Psychological Science In The Public Interest, 15(1), 3–33. doi:
- Larsen, R. J. & Zarate, M. (1991). Extending reducer/augmenter theory into the emotion domain: The role of emotion in regulating stimulation level. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 713-722.
- Nosal, C.S. (2009). The structure and regulative function of the cognitive styles: a new theory. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 41(3), 122–126. doi:
- Parasuraman, R., Warm, J.S., & Dember, W.N. (1987). Vigilance: Taxonomy and utility. In L.S.Mark, J.S.Warm, R.L.Huston. (Eds.), Ergomonics and human factors: recent research (pp. 11–32). New York: Springer.
- Ralph, B., Onderwater, K., Thomson, D., & Smilek, D. (2017). Disrupting monotony while increasing demand: Benefits of rest and intervening tasks on vigilance. Psychological Research, 81(2), 432–444. doi:
- Santostefano, S. (1971). Leveling-sharpening house test: a procedure for assessing the cognitive principle of leveling-sharpening. Boston: S. Santostefano.
- Siegel, Z.D., & Kelly, J.W. (2017). Walking through a virtual environment improves perceived size within and beyond the walked space. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(1), 39–44.
- Smit, A.S., Eling, P.A., & Coenen, A.M. (2004). Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychologica, 115, 35–42.
- Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662. doi:
- Thomson, D.R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A Resource-Control Account of Sustained Attention: Evidence From Mind-Wandering and Vigilance Paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 82–96.
- Wilson, K.E., Lowe, M.X., Ruppel, J., Pratt, J., & Ferber, S. (2016). The scope of no return: Openness predicts the spatial distribution of Inhibition of Return. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(1), 209–217.
- Zhang, L.F., Sternberg, R.J., & Rayner, S. (2012). Intellectual styles: Challenges, milestones, and agenda. In L.F. Zhang, R.J. Sternberg, & S. Rayner (Eds.), Handbook of intellectual styles: Preferences in cognition, learning, and thinking (pp. 1–20). New York: Springer Publishing Company.
Copyright information
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
About this article
Publication Date
13 December 2017
Article Doi
eBook ISBN
978-1-80296-032-7
Publisher
Future Academy
Volume
33
Print ISBN (optional)
-
Edition Number
1st Edition
Pages
1-481
Subjects
Cognitive theory, educational equipment, educational technology, computer-aided learning (CAL), psycholinguistics
Cite this article as:
Volkova, N. N., & Gusev, A. N. (2017). Do Cognitive Styles Affect Psychophysical Tasks Performance?. In S. B. Malykh, & E. V. Nikulchev (Eds.), Psychology and Education - ICPE 2017, vol 33. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 445-452). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2017.12.47