Adolescents’ Psychological Functioning And Unintentional Injuries: Motor Vehicles And Sports Accidents

Abstract

Adolescence represents a crucial period for psychological development. Adolescents have been shown more prone to injuries than adults have and they are frequently involved in motor vehicle and sports accident. Although adolescents’ injuries have been widely investigated in international literature, victims’ psychological profiles associated with different types of accidents have been scarcely addressed. The present study aimed to identify adolescents’ psychological functioning associated with different types of injuries among a population of Italian adolescents who visited an emergency department. Moreover, the present paper intended to compare psychological profiles of adolescents who have had road and sports accidents. The sample was composed by N= 60 adolescents who attended an emergency department in central Italy. Subjects completed self-report measures assessing emotional and behavioural functioning and the use of defence mechanisms. Both road and sport accidents were associated with adolescents’ maladaptive psychological functioning. Adolescents involved in motor vehicle accidents showed a problematic emotional and behavioural functioning in general and a massive use of defense strategies; their peers who had sport accidents also show various difficulties in internalizing and externalizing functioning. Several adolescents’ emotional-behavioural variables are associated with injuries rates. Risk taking behaviours and other victims’ psychological profiles should be considered to have an exhaustive picture of individual factors that can impair a healthy development.

Keywords: AdolescenceMotor vehicle accidentsSports accidents

Introduction

The developmental time window of adolescence: main properties

Adolescence represents a well-known crucial developmental phase, during which important

changes and adjustments to social, behavioural and affective-motivational functioning occur (Crone &

Dahl, 2012; Steinberg, 2008; van Duijvenvoorde, Peters, Braams & Crone, 2016). Nowadays adolescents

have to go through continuous challenges and tasks across development, including approaching unknown

environmental situations, forming new relationship, gaining independence from parents (Bornstein &

Bradley, 2014; Paciello, Fida, Cerniglia, Tramontano, & Collie, 2012).

In general, adolescents are often indicated as very vulnerable, involving in a variety of risk taking

behaviours in their everyday life (negative behaviours, delinquency, substance abuse, compensatory

eating) that in turn can lead to premature death or lifelong impairment (Crone & Dahl, 2012; Steinberg,

2008; Willoughby et al., 2013).

Recent cognitive neuroscience findings, together with research on adolescents’ brain development,

indicated various and continuous brain modifications, such as decrease in synapse, transformation at the

level of grey matter, neuronal patterns reorganization in limbic system (Petanjek et al., 2011). These

pronounced variations are very relevant in terms of emotions and thoughts’ modulation (Crone & Dahl,

2012), response to stimuli and other various areas of adolescent’s behaviours (Ernst et al., 2014;

Steinberg, 2008).

Although on the one hand all these elements lead to a potential behavioural flexibility, higher

knowledge and greater adaptive response, it may also result in adolescents’ use of maladaptive patterns

and maladaptive psychological functioning. Indeed, it was found that adolescents exhibit low levels of

risk knowledge and danger awareness when they are in different situations of everyday life. In particular,

adolescents seem to be careless and little aware about various events that they experience (Chau, 2015).

These distinguished characteristics are very common in adolescent context and they may represent

relevant injuries risk factors in this period (Chau, Kabuth & Chau, 2014). Therefore, it is not surprising

that adolescents have been shown more prone to injuries than adults: they can have up to twice-higher

injury risk, compared to adults, and they are frequently involved in motor vehicle and sports accidents

(Ramisetty-Mikler & Almakadma, 2016).

The scientific relevance of risk in adolescence is underlined by the fact that researcher and

professionals show a growing interest in investigating various aspects, which characterized this period of

life, such as positive or negative developmental patterns and maladaptive behaviours.

Adolescents’ risk taking behaviours

For aforementioned reasons, risky behaviours are very common among adolescents, who engage

in activities involving a high possibility of injuries or even death.

Indeed, adolescents, depending on their well-known sensitive personality and psychological

characteristics, can try for very dangerous experiences, showing poor ability to manage their own

activities and actions.

In fact, it was found that adolescents often and voluntary seek risky activities, because they over

valuate their capacity and ability and assume to keep the risk at a governable level: they are often in

search of emotionally strong and enthralling experiences (Figner & Weber, 2011; Zuckerman, 2007).

It also may be noted that little care about safety information is very common among adolescents,

such as in people who involve in risky experiences (Leiter & Rheinberger, 2016). Inaccurate

consciousness of risks together with a gap between real and perceived risk are also relevant in the analysis

of experience of involvement in risky behaviour (Ali, Amorim, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2009).

Many activities such as motor vehicle driving and sports engagements are very common among

adolescents, but they can represent a serious risk factor for adolescents’ psychological health. Indeed,

many common adolescents’ activities were found to be associated with a relevant risk of injury (Gabbe,

Finch, & Cameron, 2007). Depending on the various forms of risky behaviours, the outcomes may

include heavy consequences on adolescents’ health, such as behaviours and health-related problems, poor

health conditions (drug use, obesity), poor psychological health (Alink, Cicchetti, Kim, & Rogosch, 2009;

Cicchetti, 1998).

Hence, in adolescence, risk behaviour can be a crucial contributor to injuries and accidents.

In this sense, for these reasons, it is very important to understand the distinguishing characteristic

of risky behaviours, investigating the potential risk factors in adolescence. According with Chau (2015),

we strongly believe that adolescents’ injuries deserve to be investigated to provide more knowledge about

injury mechanism for future design injury prevention.

Thus, the conceptual model we adopted in the current study to explore risky behaviour and

unintentional injuries among adolescents refers to Developmental Psychopathology theory, which stresses

the contribute of individual variables and environmental factors in onset and maintaining of health-

compromising behaviours, consistent with a large amount of empirical work (Cicchetti, & Rogosch

2002).

Unintentional injuries among adolescents

Unintentional injuries represent a relevant cause of death among adolescents and the great number

of adolescents victims of accidents increasingly alarms public opinion and society.

In general, the relevance of injuries among adolescence has been acknowledged and empirically

supported (Beck, Arif, Paumier, & Jacobsen, 2016; Gorios, Souza, Gerolla, Maso, Rodrigues, & Armond,

2014; Smorti, & Guarnieri, 2014).

Researchers have traditionally defined injuries as "accidents" or unavoidable events but recently,

the understanding of the factors that can cause injuries has changed this definition; the term unintentional

injury replaces that of ‘accidental injury’ in recognition of the fact that ‘most injuries are preventable’,

with major consequences on public health (Davis, & Pless, 2001; Bonilla-Escobar, & Guiterrez, 2014).

Injury occurring is related to the specific activity concerned and the ability to deal with them.

Adolescents’ unintentional injuries, such as motor vehicle accidents and sports accidents, can have severe

repercussions for adolescents’ physical and psychological health. More specifically, motor vehicle traffic

injuries represent more than half of fatal injuries among adolescents (Blum & Qureshi, 2011), while out

of school injuries (Blum, & Qureshi, 2011) and sport accidents (Patel, Vaccaro, & Rihn 2013; Theadom

et al., 2014) are becoming an ongoing public health issue.

Among the various types of accidents involving adolescents we specifically focus on motor

vehicle accidents and on sporting accidents.

Sports accidents

Sports activities represent a widespread medium for keeping fit and achieve physical, social and

psychological benefits. Adolescents, as well as people in general, can engage in sport activities to

improve their physical and psychological well-being.

In our modern society, there is a growing interest in outdoor sport, such as mountain sports,

climbing, mountain racing, cross-country biking. Football, cycling, hockey, equestrian activities and

gymnastic are among the most popular sporting activities pursuits.

It often happens that adolescents take on the risky behaviors during sports activities. In fact, when

in sport context, participants can engage in various activities that may increase the likelihood of

potentially harmful accidents; adolescents, thus, may incur in brain injuries, spinal trauma and other

severe fractures (Patel, Vaccaro, & Rihn, 2013; Theadom et al., 2014).

Moreover, sport injuries are among the leading cause of death for American adolescents. Several

studies report high percentages of adolescents and children who incur in unintentional injuries during

enjoyment activities, such as sport (Selig, Hüpfl, Trimmel, Voelckel, & Nagele, 2012; Skokan, Junkins,

& Kadish, 2003).

Thus, the issue takes on even more complex proportions if we consider internet as related to risk

taking factor. Indeed, internet and its contents can represent a relevant source of important information for

individual interested in recreation activities. In this respect, Plank (2016), for instance, investigated risk-

taking behaviour in a context of an online outdoor sports platform, revealing a high risk associated with

adventure recreation activities and ski tour.

Motor vehicle accidents

The problem of motor vehicle accidents is very relevant when we refer to adolescence (Gorios et

al. 2014).

Motor vehicle crashes are reported to be the most relevant cause of adolescents’ fatal accidents (Sarma, Carey, Kervick, & Bimpeh, 2013). Many young people lose their lives in a very high number of

accidents, such as indicated by data from national agencies (Insurance Institute for Highway Safety,

2012) In particular, in the United States people aged between 5 and 24 years die due to motor vehicle

accidents (World Health Organization [WHO], 2014). As for Italy, road crashes are the cause of death of

many young people, especially, as reported by Marengo Marengo, Settanni, Vidotto, & Ciairano (2012) in

the case of adolescents driving motorbikes.

Problem Statement

Understanding health-endangering and maladaptive behaviours in sports and driving

Clinical and researchers should explore the field of unintentional injuries specifically addressing

individual psychological functioning and personal features.

The attention of researches has focused not only on the negative consequences but also on the risk

factors that increase the likelihood of experiencing it.

Many studies highlighted the relationship between adolescents’ personality and unintentional

accidents (Galovsky, Malta, & Blanchard, 2006; Marengo, Settanni, Vidotto, & Ciairano, 2012), together

with various constellation of psychological aspects, such as depression, anxiety and impulsivity (Paciello,

Fida, Cerniglia, Tramontano, & Collie, 2012). It is known that the construct of sensation seeking plays a

central role in risk taking behaviours, in the aspect of temperamental predisposition: it refers to a seeking

after relevant experiences from the emotional point of view in a context in which individual’s poor

awareness of risk may increase the risk of negative behaviours (Zuckerman, 2007; Steinberg, 2008).

However, as well highlighted by Cerniglia et al. (2015), the majority of studies on adolescents’

psychological functioning and unintentional injuries has concentrated more on psychopathological

consequences that occur after the crashes and collisions, than on the psychological and emotional aspects

preceding the road accidents.

About this, Carbone (2009; 2010) stated that adolescents’ maladaptive emotional functioning, e.g.

alexithymic traits, and other dysfunctional psychological functioning, such as a massive use of defense

strategies, are related to the high-risk behaviours. In fact, these particular maladaptive patterns, when

preceding the road accidents, can lead to negative consequences that we see in severe injuries and

collisions. In this perspective, adolescents’ accidents seem to be an unconscious psychological help

request.

Research Questions

The majority of researches on unintentional injuries have paid much attention to the role of

environmental, individual and psychopathological variables, whereas little attention has been given to an

exploration of emotional and psychological factors related to injuries among adolescence.

More specifically, although the rates of adolescents’ injuries have been widely investigated in

international literature, there is still a lack of data on the victims’ psychological profiles. Few studies, to

our knowledge, have examined psychological and emotional variable related to unintentional injuries

(Cerniglia et al., 2015) and to date we have no data about psychological functioning of adolescents who

get involved in different types of accidents.

Thus, it is crucial to identify adolescents’ psychological profiles in the context of unintentional

injuries, also taking into account the main frequent accident scenarios. Only a detailed knowledge of the

primary causes and contributory factors (in term of individual and relational features related to the various

forms of accidents) may lead to realize effective program of prevention and intervention.

Purpose of the Study

Taking into account the aforementioned considerations, the current study intends to report a

contribution towards the understanding of adolescents’ unintentional injuries, considering them as form

of psychological help request, which do not necessarily overlap with irreparable consequences.

Thus, the purpose of the present study was to investigate emotional psychological profiles of

adolescents involved in unintentional injuries. More precisely, we aimed to verify whether some

adolescents’ unintentional injuries, such as motor vehicle accidents and sports accidents, are related to

specific psychological features, such as emotional difficulties and use of defense strategies.

Moreover, we wanted to assess the adolescents’ psychological functioning associated with

different types of injuries specifically among a population of Italian adolescents who visited an

emergency department. A parallel goal of our study was to compare psychological profiles of adolescents

who had road and sports accidents.

Since emotional competence has an important role in the adaptive psychological functioning by

which adolescents attempt their environmental situations, we hypothesized that unintentional injuries

among adolescents can be related to maladaptive psychological profiles and psychological sufferance.

More specifically, we hypothesize that personnel difficulties in identifying and describing

emotions and use of defensive strategies characterize adolescents’ psychological functioning when

involved in unintentional injuries.

In other words, we expected that:

Adolescents admitted to emergency departments due to unintentional injuries reveal difficulties in

identifying and describing their own emotions;

Adolescents admitted to emergency departments due to unintentional injuries report massive use

defense strategies;

Adolescents specifically involved in motor vehicle and sports accidents can show specific

emotional psychological profiles, such as alexithymic traits and use of defensive mechanisms.

Research Methods

Subjects and procedure

We proceeded as follows.

In order to carry out our research, a sample of adolescents who attended an emergency department,

located in cities of Italy, was enrolled.

We selected N= 58 adolescents (total males: 32; total females: 26; age range: 14–23 years) who

visited an Italian emergency department over a period of one year (from May 2015 to May 2016) and

who were involved in various type of accidents. The mean age of the subjects was 17.72 (s.d. = 2.56).

Participants took part in this study, with two different groups of adolescents being involved,

according to the type of accident they had: Group 1: adolescents that have suffered sports accidents (N =

29; 16 boys and 13 girls); Group 2: adolescents that have suffered motor vehicle accidents (N = 29; 16

boys and 13 girls).

Participants in the study were recruited while they were in the emergency department. The

majority of them were Caucasian. All subjects came from families with middle or high socioeconomic

status. All participants were told about the goals of this study. They were voluntary and anonymous.

Subjects completed data on all study variables (self-reported instruments assessing emotional

functioning and use of defense strategies) and none of those who accepted dropped the task.

Participation time was approximately 45 minutes and trained psychologists administered the

instruments.

Prior permission was obtained from the Ethical Committee of the Medicine and Psychology

Faculty at Sapienza University of Rome, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Measures

Assessment of the ability to describe and identify emotions

We used one of the most frequently applied measure on ability to describe and identify emotions:

The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) developed by Bagby, Parker, & Taylor (Bagby, Parker,

&Taylor, 1994; Italian version La Ferlita, Bonadies, Solano, De Gennaro, & Gonini, 2007).

The TAS-20 is a self-report scale comprising 20 Items for emotional functioning. Respondents

were asked to indicate on a 5 point scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree); five items are

negatively keyed.

Scores for the subscales are obtained by summing the response categories selected by the

participants. The scale presents a three-factor structure congruent with the alexithymia construct. The first

factor consists of items that assess the ability to identify and to distinguish feelings. The second factor

comprise items measuring the ability to describe feelings. The third factor includes items assessing

externally oriented thinking.

Higher score reflects increasing levels of more maladaptive emotional functioning.

The instrument shows a good internal consistency (Cronbach’s ɑ 0.86).

Assessment of defense strategies

Use of defense strategies was measured using The Response Evaluation Measure for Youth

(REM-71; Steiner, Araujo, & Koopman, 2001; Italian version by Prunas et al., 2009): the REM-71 is a

self-report scale comprising 71-items that assess a series of defenses listed as following: Acting out,

Splitting, Displacement, Dissociation, Fantasy, Passive aggression, Projection, Repression, Omnipotence,

Undoing, Conversion, Somatization, Withdrawal, Suppression, Denial, Humor, Intellectualization,

Reaction Formation, Idealization, Altruism, Sublimation.

Questions to be answered are scored on a 9-point scale from ‘‘strongly disagree” (scored as 1) to

‘‘strongly agree” (scored as 9).

Higher scores on this measure reflect a more massive and frequent use of the defense strategies.

Statistical analysis

Before performing the analyses, a preliminary screening of the data was conducted and only a few

data items were missing for each instrument.

To evaluate the adolescents’ emotional profiles and the use of defense strategies in the two groups,

multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) was carried out, considering age and gender.

All analyses were performed with SPSS software (Version 22.0).

Findings

Adolescents’ alexithymic traits emerged from the current study, with participants showing

difficulties in identifying and describing their emotions, together with use of defense strategies.

Assessment of the ability to describe and identify emotions

To verify whether adolescents involved in motor vehicle, sports accidents show difficulties in

recognizing and describing their emotions, a MANOVA on the two group scores on all subscales of TAS-

20 was carried out, considering the effect of age and gender. Group 1: adolescents who have experienced

sports accidents (N = 29). Group 2: adolescents who have experienced motor vehicle accidents (N = 29).

Adolescents’ scores on all TAS 20’s scales were higher, with the majority of scores indicating a

possible presence of alexithymia. N = 2 subjects in Group 1 and N = 2 subjects in Group 2 exceed the

clinical cut-off at TAS-20’s total scores.

No significant difference between group 1 and group 2 on the TAS 20’s subscales was found (λ =

9.73; F = .250; p .861). Moreover, Group 1 and Group 2 have been shown to have difference based on

gender as regard adolescents’ scores on Factor 3 (F = 9.437; p ˂ .005).

Assessment of defense strategies

To verify whether adolescents involved in motor vehicle and sports accidents strongly use defense

strategies we conducted a MANOVA on the two group scores on all subscales of REM 71. MANOVA

conducted on the two groups’ scores showed no significant differences between adolescents belonging to

Group 1 and adolescents belonging to Group 2 a regard to the use of defense strategies, except on

Projection (F = 27.70; p < .05) Conversion (F = 32.79; p < .001), Altruism (F = 14.53; p <.001).

In general, although no significant differences in scores on the subscales of the TAS-20 and 71

REM were found among adolescents who had experienced motor vehicle accidents and sports accidents,

data indicated that adolescents who have turned to an emergency department, due to unintentional

injuries, tend to manifest difficulty in identifying and qualifying their emotions, also using defensive

strategies. More precisely, regardless of the specific type of accidents suffered, the fact of incurring an

unintentional injury can be read as a signal of distress and individual suffering during adolescence. In

accord with Carbone (2009; 2010), indeed, accidents among adolescents can be interpreted as an

expression of deep psychological distress, implying an unspoken help request.

Overall, results obtained evidenced that both road and sport accidents occurred to our sample

subjects are related to adolescents’ psychological suffering. Thus, our findings generally support the

hypothesis of this study.

In line with previous research, risk taking behaviours and injuries among adolescents proved to be

related to general suffering, maladaptive psychological patterns and disadvantaged individual.

These results suggest that risk behaviour tends to be related to maladaptive responses that are

established by adolescents to face up their emotional difficulties.

Finally, according to our data, victims’ psychological profiles should be considered to have an

exhaustive picture of individual factors that can impair a healthy development.

Conclusion

The main aim of this study was to investigate psychological functioning within adolescents who

were involved in unintentional injuries.

We expected that adolescents who incurred motor vehicle accidents and sports accidents would

have shown difficulties in coping with their emotions as well as we expected that they would have turned

to a massive use of defense strategies.

Overall, the results are consistent with previous research (Cerniglia et al., 2015), showing both in

males and females, high levels of alexithyimic traits among adolescents who have had unintentional

injuries, suggesting a significant impairment of psychological well-being, as well as physical, resulting in

accidents.

Specifically, our findings reveal that almost all participants experienced some form of

psychological maladaptive functioning, regardless of the type of accident. Moreover, the results of the

present study show also in our sample a high prevalence of psychological suffering and emotional

difficulties related to the number of accidents actually occurred (Cerniglia et al., 2015; Erriu, 2016).

These data support the notion that having difficulties or poor emotional competence might have a

key role in facing the daily challenges and tasks among adolescents. We do know that emotional and

behavioural difficulties during developmental age are potential sources of maladaptive emotional

functioning in adulthood (Cimino, Cerniglia, & Paciello, 2015; Cimino, Cerniglia, Paciello, & Sinesi,

2013).

For these reasons, our results, combined with previous findings of Cerniglia et al. (2015), would

seem to suggest that it is crucial understanding psychological conditions in adolescents, particularly their

experienced feelings. Indeed, most personal difficulties are modifiable for prevention and intervention

(Nilsson, Stigson, Ohlin, & Strandroth, 2017; Salam et al., 2016) and many problems can be solved

through an early assessment and treatment nowadays.

Our results should be viewed in the light of several methodological limitations.

The assessment of adolescents’ psychological functioning was based on self-report measures, so

that the current study is based on reports from adolescents at one point in time rather than teacher or

parents or peers. We believe that it would be advisable to employ different sources, objective measures

and alternative methodologies such as individual interviews.

Moreover, the cross sectional design of our study cannot allow a longitudinal assessment of

psychological functioning across the development. Thus, future research should add different evaluation

times to better explore the experience of emotions in adolescents overtime and adolescents

psychopathological outcomes.

Another important limitation could be attributed to the small sample size, as the study could be

underpowered to detect small differences. Indeed, our sample size limits the generalizability of the

current study. The findings should be confirmed in a future study involving a greater number of

participants.

Hence, firm conclusions about the adolescents’ psychological profiles in the context of

unintentional injuries cannot be made: it appears necessary then to replicate and extend the results in

order to overcome these methodological problems.

Despite the aforementioned limitations, the current study adds to our knowledge about

adolescents’ psychological functioning, providing information to better investigate such an important

period of life as the adolescent.

Adolescents should be helped to be conscious of their problems and our research can be used to

develop interventions aimed to support them, considering the need of more empirical studies in Italy.

References

  1. Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 758–762.
  2. Alink, L. R., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2009). Mediating and moderating processes in the relation between maltreatment and psychopathology: Mother-child relationship quality and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(6), 831-843.
  3. Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23–32.
  4. Beck, N. I., Arif, I., Paumier, M. F., & Jacobsen, K. H. (2016). Adolescent injuries in Argentina, Bolivia, Chile, and Uruguay: Results from the 2012–2013 Global School-based Student Health Survey (GSHS). Injury, 47(12), 2642-2649.
  5. Blum, R. W., & Qureshi, F. (2011). Morbidity and mortality among adolescents and young adults in the United States. Baltimore, MD.
  6. Bonilla-Escobar, F. J., & Gutiérrez, M. I. (2014). Injuries are not accidents: towards a culture of prevention. Colombia medica, 45(3), 132-135.
  7. Bornstein, M.H. & Bradley, R.H. (2014). Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development. London: Routledge.
  8. Carbone, P. (2009). Le ali di Icaro; capire e prevenire gli incidenti dei giovani. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri.
  9. Carbone, P. (2010). L’Adolescente prende corpo. Roma: II Pensiero Scientifico Editore.
  10. Cerniglia, L., Cimino, S., Ballarotto, G., Casini, E., Ferrari, A., Carbone, P., & Cersosimo, M. (2015).
  11. Motor vehicle accidents and adolescents: an empirical study on their emotional and behavioral profiles, defense strategies and parental support. Transportation research part F: traffic psychology and behaviour, 35, 28-36.
  12. Chau, K. (2015). Impact of sleep difficulty on single and repeated injuries in adolescents. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 81, 86-95.
  13. Chau, K., Kabuth, B., & Chau, N. (2014). Gender and family disparities in suicide attempt and role of socioeconomic, school, and health-related difficulties in early adolescence. BioMed research international, 2014.
  14. Cicchetti, D. A. N. T. E. (2008). A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on research in development and psychopathology. Child and adolescent psychopathology, 27-57.
  15. Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 70(1), 6.
  16. Cimino, S., Cerniglia, L., Paciello, M., & Sinesi, S. (2013). A Six-year Prospective Study on Children of Mothers with Eating Disorders: The Role of Paternal Psychological Profiles. European Eating Disorders Review, 21, 238-246.
  17. Cimino, S., Cerniglia, L., & Paciello, M. (2015). Mothers with depression, anxiety or eating disorders: Outcomes on their children and the role of paternal psychological profiles. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46, 228-236.
  18. Crone, E. A. & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat Rev Neurosci. 13(9), 636-50. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313.
  19. Davis, R. M., & Pless, B. (2001). BMJ bans" accidents". British Medical Journal, 322(7298), 1320-1320. Ernst, A., Alkass, K., Bernard, S., Salehpour, M., Perl, S., Tisdale, J., Possnert, G., Druid, H., Frisén', J.
  20. (2014). Neurogenesis in the Striatum of the Adult Human Brain. Cell., 156(5), 1072–1083. DOI: DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.01.044.
  21. Erriu, M. (2016). Emotional–Behavioural Profiles and Parental Support in Adolescents with Motor Vehicle Accidents. The European Proceedings of Social & Behavioural Sciences EpSBS, 13, 303-312. DOI: 10.15405/epsbs.2016.07.02.30.
  22. Figner, B., & Weber, E. U. (2011). Who takes risks when and why? Determinants of risk taking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(4), 211-216.
  23. Gabbe, B. J., Finch, C. F., & Cameron, P. A. (2007). Priorities for reducing the burden of injuries in sport: the example of Australian football. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 10(5), 273-276.
  24. Galovski, T. E., Malta, L. S., & Blanchard, E. B. (2006). Road rage: Assessment and treatment of the angry, aggressive driver. American Psychological Association.
  25. Gorios, C., Souza, R. M. D., Gerolla, V., Maso, B., Rodrigues, C. L., & Armond, J. D. E. (2014).
  26. Transport accidents among children and adolescents at the emergency service of a teaching hospital in the southern zone of the city of São Paulo. Revista Brasileira de Ortopedia, 49(4), 391-395.
  27. Insurance Institute for Highway Safety (IIHS) (2012). Fatality facts: Teenagers 2012. Arlington (VA): The Institute. Retrieved from: http://www.iihs.org/iihs/topics/ t/teenagers/fatalityfacts/teenagers. La Ferlita, V., Bonadies, M., Solano, L., De Gennaro, L., & Gonini, P. (2007). Alessitimia e adolescenza: studio preliminare di validazione della TAS-20 su un campione di 360 adolescenti italiani. Infanzia e Adolescenza, 6(3), 131–144.
  28. Leiter, A. M., & Rheinberger, C. M. (2016). Risky sports and the value of safety information. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 131, 328-345.
  29. Marengo, D., Settanni, M., Vidotto, G., & Ciairano, S. (2012). Profili di personalità e rischio stradale in adolescenza: uno studio longitudinale in ambiente simulato e reale. Ricerche di Psicologia.
  30. Nilsson, P., Stigson, H., Ohlin, M., & Strandroth, J. (2017). Modelling the effect on injuries and fatalities when changing mode of transport from car to bicycle. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 100, 30-36. Paciello, M., Fida, R., Cerniglia, L., Tramontano, C., & Collie, E. (2012). High cost helping scenario: the role of empathy, prosocial reasoning and moral disengagement on helping behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(1), 3–7. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.004.
  31. Paciello, M., Fida, R., Tramontano, C., Collie, E., & Cerniglia, L. (2012). Moral dilemma in adolescence: The role of values, prosocial moral reasoning and moral disengagement in helping decisionmaking. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(2), 190–205. DOI: 10.1080/ 17405629.2012.759099.
  32. Patel, S. A., Vaccaro, A. R., & Rihn, J. A. (2013). Epidemiology of spinal injuries in sports. Operative Techniques in Sports Medicine, 21(3), 146-151.
  33. Petanjek, Z., Judaš, M., Šimić, G., Rašin, M. R., Uylings, H. B., Rakic, P., & Kostović, I. (2011).
  34. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), 13281-13286.
  35. Plank, A. (2016). The hidden risk in user-generated content: An investigation of ski tourers' revealed risk-taking behavior on an online outdoor sports platform. Tourism Management, 55, 289-296.
  36. Prunas, A., Madeddu, F., Pozzoli, S., Gatti, C., Shaw, R. J., & Steiner, H. (2009). The Italian version of the response evaluation measure-71. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(4), 369–377.
  37. Ramisetty-Mikler, S., & Almakadma, A. (2016). Attitudes and behaviors towards risky driving among adolescents in Saudi Arabia. International Journal of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 3(2), 55-63.
  38. Salam, R. A., Arshad, A., Das, J. K., Khan, M. N., Mahmood, W., Freedman, S. B., & Bhutta, Z. A.
  39. (2016). Interventions to prevent unintentional injuries among adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescent Health, 59(4), S76-S87.
  40. Sarma, K. M., Carey, R. N., Kervick, A. A., & Bimpeh, Y. (2013). Psychological factors associated with indices of risky, reckless and cautious driving in a national sample of drivers in the Republic of Ireland. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 50, 1226-1235.
  41. Selig, H. F., Hüpfl, M., Trimmel, H., Voelckel, W. G., & Nagele, P. (2012). Pediatric trauma in the Austrian Alps: The epidemiology of sport-related injuries in Helicopter Emergency Medical Service. High altitude medicine & biology, 13(2), 112-117.
  42. Skokan, E. G., Junkins, E. P., & Kadish, H. (2003). Serious winter sport injuries in children and adolescents requiring hospitalization. The American journal of emergency medicine, 21(2), 95-99. Smorti, M., & Guarnieri, S. (2014). Sensation seeking, parental bond, and risky driving in adolescence: Some relationships, matter more to girls than boys. Safety science, 70, 172-179.
  43. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106.
  44. Steiner, H., Araujo, K. B., & Koopman, C. (2001). The Response Evaluation Measure (REM-71): A new instrument for the measurement of defenses in adult and adolescent. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 467–473.
  45. Theadom, A., Starkey, N. J., Dowell, T., Hume, P. A., Kahan, M., McPherson, K., ... & BIONIC Research Group. (2014). Sports-related brain injury in the general population: an epidemiological study. Journal of science and medicine in sport, 17(6), 591-596.
  46. van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K., Peters, S., Braams, B. R., Crone, E.A. (2016). What Motivates Adolescents? Neural Responses to Rewards and Their Influence on Adolescents' Risk Taking, DOI: Learning, and Cognitive Control, Neurosci Biobehav Rev., 70, 135-147.
  47. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.037.
  48. World Health Organization (30 August 2014). Global health observatory (GHO). Retrieved from: http://www.who.int/gho/ mortality_burden_disease/causes_death_2008/en/index.html.
  49. Willoughby, T., Good, M., Adachi P. J.C., Hamza, C., Tavernier R. (2013). Examining the link between adolescent brain development and risk taking from a social–developmental perspective. Brain and Cognition, 83, 315–323.
  50. Zuckerman, M. (2007). The sensation seeking scale V (SSS-V): Still reliable and valid. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(5), 1303-1305.

Copyright information

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

About this article

Publication Date

12 June 2017

eBook ISBN

978-1-80296-023-5

Publisher

Future Academy

Volume

24

Print ISBN (optional)

-

Edition Number

1st Edition

Pages

1-91

Subjects

Sports, sport science, physical education

Cite this article as:

Erriu, M., Ballarotto, G., Marzilli, E., Cerniglia, L., & Cimino, S. (2017). Adolescents’ Psychological Functioning And Unintentional Injuries: Motor Vehicles And Sports Accidents. In E. Lupu, G. Niculescu, & E. Sabău (Eds.), Sport, Education & Psychology - icSEP 2017, vol 24. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 1-12). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2017.06.1