Motor Coordination and Reactivity Influenced by Mental Training in Alpine Skiing

Abstract

Problem statement:The purpose of this research is to show that the implementation of mental training techniques improves the performance in alpine skiing. Scientific originality is determined by the adaptation and implementation of a mental training program for skiers of 12-19 years, children, small and big juniors, consisting in a complex technique and psychological preparation program, during three competition years (October 2012 - March 2015). Aim: The goal of this research is to identify certain psychological factors that will help along with the relevant physical ones in obtaining skier’s performance behaviour and achieving sport performance; especially to increase in the capacity of motor coordination and reactivity response in certain alpine skiers. Practical value of the paper is characterised by the development of an experimental model that includes the way in which the structure is implemented and the development of the program’s content in ski sport training, accomplishment of all the required techniques and enhancement in the technique of mountain ski descending, in both series. Conclusions: The difference between the results is due to our intervention consisting in applying mental imagery in the most important technical elements within alpine skiing. In order to teach athletes certain skills to control their own study behaviour, one needs knowledge and abilities concerning the processes and mechanisms that play a direct role in personal management.

Keywords: Mental trainingalpine skiersmotor coordinationreactivity actionmotor development

1. Introduction

We used theCMR test (motor coordination and reactivity) for determining the motor coordination

capacity and the reactivity of alpine skiers, as well as the mental control of fatigue. In order to

determine motor coordination and reactivity, psychomotor capacity in alpine skiing, in this research we

used a device called Computer – assisted, created by Management Design SRL Iasi, (Hãvârneanu,

2007). This device provides the possibility of obtaining indicators of information. According to the

term “vigilance” as understood by Bonnardel (Hăvârneanu, 2011), we study some components of

coordination like: execution and self-regulation capacity – indicators of safe behaviour.

The process of self-regulated learning is assimilated to the interaction between several factors –

personal (motivational, strategic and related to self-control), behavioural and contextual – with an

impact upon the athletic performances of students (Zimmerman, 2002).

Especially when the sportsman fails to achieve a certain level of competition, mental imagery is

used to focus on an image of desired reality, to develop techniques abilities. Certain strategies aim to

obtain not only high performances, but also the development of autonomous learning capacity (of

planning activities by priorities, of mobilizing oneself to attain the objectives, of organizing the

learning process and the context of study, etc).

The development of self-regulating abilities can be achieved within the instructive process, by

increasing the importance of certain activities, cited by Crotty that mediate the experiences enabling the

athlete to: explore systematically a learning situation; self-monitor the cognitive processes, the

learning activity per se and the means of organizing the knowledge; plan the learning behaviour; reflect

upon the personal method of learning/ understanding; generate personal meanings, which will represent

filters for interpreting the study material.

Within new tasks, they generally cover the following steps (Tirri & Nokelainen, 2011): analyse the

task and interpret its requirements; determine objectives specific to task fulfilling; select and adapt

strategies by these objectives; monitor the progress made for attaining the proposed purposes; adjust

the strategies and effort made after monitoring; use motivational strategies to persist in the task when

encountering difficulties or the presence of distracters.

2. Purpose of study

The aim of this study was to track changes and overall coordination components after applying

mental training techniques. Of these components we can include various indicators that were tested at

the beginning and end of the experiment in both groups: control and experimental. These indicators

were measured by the following: IM – motor learning; CMC – movement coordination; ER – reaction

accuracy; RR – reaction speed; CMD – dissociate movement synchronization; CMS – associate –

dissociate movement synchronization; AR – self-regulation .(Grosu & Grosu, 2015)

In the experimental group, mental training techniques were applied, but not in the control group.

The difference between the results is due to our intervention consisting in applying mental imagery in

the most important technical elements within alpine skiing, (Akyürek, Schubö, & Hommel, 2013).

Executive functioning represents an umbrella concept that circumscribes the high level coordination

of a series of cognitive processes used for regulating and directing behaviour towards the purpose, in

new or difficult situations (Hughes and Graham, 2002).

According to Grosu (2012), by following proprioceptive and kinaesthetic processes, specific to

the mental training, we can increase the ability to combine and recombine mental images.

In literature there are different terms that refer to mental training. Among them we can mention:

mental rehearsal, practice mental visualization and repetition representation imagery. (Grosu,1999).

After applying the mental training techniques in the experimental group, we also observed

significant changes on the motor coordination parameters and reactivity.

The applied tests aim to observe several aspects of the speed and precision of the perception

together with the efficiency of the operational thinking. The information regarding reactivity are filled

by observing the speed reaction and time reaction (Egan, D., 1996)

During the mental training interview we have asked our athletes to five us more information about

the movement techniques in alpine skiing.

We started with mental training on pivot turn. A ski on the edge in direct falling starts a triple

movement in the plane of the field: translation; passive pivoting uphill, due to a couple of forces:

braking force at the spatula level and pressing force on the centre of the ski, provided by its weight;

pivoting (bending) around its own longitudinal axis, which augments the effect of uphill pivoting by

increasing the braking on spatula level. (Grosu, 2015) The aforementioned aspects concern the support

of the longitudinal ski profile (an arch made by the two lateral margins). “The piloting” of skis on the

loop arch depends on maintaining a curve-like trajectory of the skis; it is mainly the effect of the

relation between the centrifugal and the centripetal force. The mechanisms of the rotational gestures

made by the skier are supported by the coxo-femoral, knee, and ankle joints, which enable two types of

pivoting: around the vertical axis, represented by the tibia, (adduction and abduction movements) and

around the sole, (pronation and supination). In flexion, the pivot turn is obtained by the conjugated

intervention of the quadriceps that fixes the flexion angle and of the traction executed by the posterior

thigh muscles (internal and external rotator muscles, which also fix the thigh on the sides for

transmitting the pivot movement dictated by the hips and for maintaining the ski on the edge).

The first four stimuli are manually administered: Ms, Md, Ps, Pd. Observation: depending on the

case, the subject will be told to press just once and then to wait for the first stimulus to disappear and

for the subsequent one to emerge. The wrong reaction is pinpointed in both visual and auditory terms

(“Wrong!”). After providing explanations, the examiner will repeat the stimulus. “As follows, you will

see two yellow rectangles: one for the hand and one for the leg. (Grosu, 2015a)You must push the

corresponding buttons, at the same time if it is possible. Attention!” The four stimuli are manually

administered: MsPs – left hand/ left leg, MdPd – right hand / right leg, MsPd – left hand/ right leg,

MdPs – right hand/ left leg. The subsequent part of the drill comprises series of eight paired-stimuli

each, administered automatically. Upon each wrong reaction, the stimulus is repeated until the right

reaction is achieved, then the series is reprised. It is considered that the subject managed to adjust when

he completes an entire series without error.

3. Methodology and results

For the purpose of this study we have requested and received the approval from the Etic Committee

in accordance with Helsinki Declaration. Moreover, we have also received the acceptance of the tutors

of the athletes who participated in the experiment.

There is also informed opinion of the subjects participating in research. Subjects received notice

underage parents to participate in research.

Research design : The experiment took place at different places: Cluj-Napoca, Gheorgheni and

Baia-Sprie between 1.10. 2014 – 31.03.2015. Subjects and groupsconstituted byjunior athletes in

alpine skiing with age old between 11 ± 5 and 15 ± 3 years. One group was from the sportive clubs:

CSS Baia-Sprie, Maramureș District, CSS Gheorgheni, Harghita district. The other group was form by

juniors from different ski clubs: CS Sinaia (PH), CS Predeal (BV), C Crazy – Bike Sibiu (SB), C Piatra

– Neamt (NT), CS Miercurea-Ciuc (HR).

We applied the CMR test (motor coordination and reactivity) test. These initial tests were used as

starting points in our research, in which we want to increase sports performance through the application

of mental training techniques. The assumption of this research: by applying a mental training

programme on the selected group, the athletes were capable to maintain their calm, mobilize the energy

during the competition and capable of self-control. We have also observed that the athletes know how

to define the control sensation.

To accomplish the statistical work we have applied the SPSS 15.0 software. As a process we have

made the comparison of averages between subjects and applied ANOVA to all indicators: AI, CMC,

ER, RR, CMD, CMS and AR during the pre-test measurements. On both groups the results were

similar before the experiment.

The intervention programme consists in applying several imagination exercises on specific

components of the downhill driving in alpine skiing, based on general coordination. We have also

calculated the t test on the specific groups both before and after the intervention program in order to

observe if any change occurred.

The results ranged between 1 and 5, where 1 = very low value, and 5= very high value, in order to

pinpoint possible differences between the control group and the experimental group. The female athlete

L.E. finished first at the National Championship of School Sports Clubs (CSS) organized in March

2015. She is a very good athlete; at four indicators (CMC), (CMD), (AR), she scored 4 out of 5; and at

(RR), she scored 5.

Table 1 -
See Full Size >

Results show that the intervention had effects on the experimental group compared to the control

group in case of IM t (46) = 3.206, p = 0.02<0.05, with average effect value of 0.42 and CMD t (46) =

3.275 p = 0.2<0.5, with average effect value of 0.43. For the other CMC, ER, RR, CMS, and AR, we

cannot posit that it was our effect and not pure chance, because the differences between the two groups

are not statistically significant (see Table 2 ).

Table 2 -
See Full Size >

4. Discussions and findings

We investigated the following variables (Hăvârneanu, 2011). Motor coordination, which involves

the assessment criteria of global test results, and the following indicators: total Correct Reactions

(CMC) and Synchronisation (CMS). Mobility, which involves the assessment criteria of Easiness of

acquiring the stereotype, Working tempo, and Self-regulation, and the following indicators: number of

stimuli required for adaptation (IM), Percentage of omissions, and Resume after error (AR), (Frank,

Land, & Schack, 2013).

Both types of pivot turns serve the mechanisms of boot canting/decanting; both associate, generally,

to those of the knee. The muscles that cause these pivot turns have a limited power; they are effective

when skis are on their edges and their edges have the role of lateral fixation: 1. the spine joints are

mobilized – during pivoting – in the same sense as the pivoting of leg joints – rotations (Grigoraș,

2013), or vice-versa - laevorotations; 2. the lumbosacral joint - the pivot turns of the joint are almost

always associated with the pivot movements of L5, L4, or L3; sometimes, pivoting at this level totally

replaces thigh pivoting (Fellows, 2011).

Balance with reactivity control, with assessment criterion. Indicators: Percentage of omissions in

relation to the number of stimuli required for adaptation and the percentage of omissions related to the

number of omissions made downhill and Behaviour in intense stimulation situations (especially on V3

speed). As you move ahead, internalize the motions and visualize the action of the carving ski (Dilts,

Epstein, & Dilts, 2011).

Operational thought efficiency, with the following assessment criteria (Egan, 1996). Learning

capacity and the progress made after the drill. Indicators: Understanding the assignment and Number of

stimuli required for adaptation (IM). So far the only fundamental element of high performance alpine

skiing remains the equipment technique and the execution techniques. (Master, 2010)

Especially, when the athlete does not manage to execute technically a movement he is asked to

concentrate on a key aspect that triggers the specific movement (this can be either an image or the

entire aspect). Accordign to some researchers (Unsworth, McMillan, Brewer & Spillers, 2012), elite

athletes are characterized by the following qualities: they are more devoted, more motivated and have

more self-esteem and know how to concentrate on what is essential or not. These athletes know how to

overcome the obstacles and compete with maximum efficiency in stress conditions. Others researchers

created specific surveys to test the athletes.

In order to test their mental abilities keeping into account different competition and sports standards

(Schack, & Hackfort, 2007). After them opinion, psychological variables and abilities which consist the

base of top performances, were examined mostly through qualitative interviews or through a

combination of questionnaires and interviews. Specialists can use such information for planning,

implementing and optimization of psychological interventions, helping this way expert sportsmen and

also novices to achieve the highest possible level (Filho, & et. al., 2015; Sadeghi, & et. al., 2010).

Strategies are learning techniques used intentionally in order to reach certain objectives. An

important aspect of these strategic programs is represented by information concerning when and how

one must use newly acquired strategies (Schraw and Brooks, 1999).

5. Conclusions

By analysing the scores of the tests applied on the selected groups, we can say that the mental

training through mental imagery can improve the results of the athletes, especially in the components:

IM – motor learning (number of stimuli required for adaptation) and CMD – coordination of

movements – synchronization (sync indicator dissociated movements)

In the experimental group we have observed an increase of the performance as it results from the

analysis of the competitions that took place in session: Dec 2014 - March 2015. We can also point out

the role and objective of the mental representation by using mental images together with sensations and

perceptions. These come along with the movement during the imagery moment in order to execute the

mental technique closer to the real action.

Consequently, the skills enumerated below differentiate students who self-regulate the learning

process from those who do not (Boekaerts, M., Corno 2005, Zimmerman, 2002): familiarization with a

series of cognitive strategies, knowing when and how to use them (through repetition, organization,

elaboration and organization); organization, control, directing mental process towards attaining

personal goals; the presence of a set of motivational beliefs and adaptive emotions, such as the

following: high sense of obtaining school efficiency; establishing clear learning objectives; channelling

positive emotions towards fulfilling certain tasks (e.g., joy, satisfaction, enthusiasm); planning and

control of time and resources necessary to initiate and finalize a task; mobilization and effort to control

and regulate training tasks; application of voluntary strategies.

Cognitive concern both mental processes and specific behaviours of students while acquiring new

knowledge. The adjustment of cognitive strategies to the current task is the result of the so-called

procedural knowledge, namely the knowledge of the most effective techniques for acquiring and

assimilating notions (MacLean, 1997).

Conflicts of interests

We declare no conflicts of interest. Research capitalizes partial results from the PhD thesis of the

first author, presented on 18 June 2015, at the "Babeș - Bolyai" Cluj-Napoca, Faculty of Physical

Education and Sport.

Acknowledgment

We are very grateful to the skiers and ski trainers who participated in our experiment.

References

  1. Akyürek, E., Schubö, A, Hommel, B. (2013). Attentional control and competition between episodic representations. Psychological Research, Jul, 77(4), 492-507.
  2. Boekaerts, M., Corno. L.B. (2005). Self – Regulation in the Classroom: A Perspective on Assessment and Intervention. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 54(2), 199-231.
  3. Dilts, R.D., Epstein, T., & Dilts, R.W. (2011). NLP and the creativity. Rome, Italy: Edit. Alessio Roberti. Egan, D. (1996). All-Terrain skiing, Instructions, Drills, Exercises, U.S.Military, and Special Sales by World Leisure Corporation, 177 Paris Street, Boston, MA 02128.
  4. Fellows, C. (2011). Total Skiing, The proven pyramid approach for improving: functional movement, fitness, techniques, tactics. PO Box 5076 Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  5. Filho, E., di Fronso, S., Forzini, F., Murgia, M., Agostini, T., Bortoli, L., Robazza, C., Bertollo, M. (2015).
  6. Athletic performance and recovery stress factors in cycling: An ever changing balance. Eur. J Sport Sci., DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1048746.
  7. Frank, C., Land, W.M., & Schack, T. (2013). Development of mental representations during learning of a complex action. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14, 353- 361.
  8. Grigoraș, P. (2013). Învățarea motorie în schiul alpin. Baze teoretice și repere practice. Cluj-Napoca: Risoprint. Grosu, E.F. (1999). Aplicatii ale antrenamentului mental în sport, Acţiune motorie versus acţiune psihică, vol. 3, GMI Press, ISBN 973-98721-3-1.
  9. Grosu, E.F. (2012). Tehnici de antrenament mental. GMI Press, (3rd edition), ISBN 978-973-1776-40-8, ISBN 978-973-1776-41-5.
  10. Grosu, E.F., Grosu, V.T. (2015). Influence of Mental Training on Motor Coordination and Manual Response in Alpine Skiing. International Journal of Modern Education Research, 2(5), 57-61 Grosu, V.T. (2015). Optimizarea performanței în schiul alpin prin antrenament mental, imagerie și programare neurolingvistică. Teza de doctorat susținută la Univ. Babeș – Bolyai, Cluj – Napoca, mai 2015. (Conducător prof. univ. dr. Monea Gh.) Grosu, V.T. (2015a). Ghid pentru practicarea schiului alpin. Cluj – Napoca: Editura GMI.
  11. Hăvârneanu, C. (2011). Evaluarea psihologică a conducătorilor auto. Romania: “Alexandru Ioan Cuza” University Press Iaşi, ISBN 978-973-703-633-9.
  12. Hăvârneanu, E.C. (2007). Psitest Cabinet, Sistem informatizat pentru evaluarea în psihologia transporturilor și a muncii, Versiunea 04/2007, Iași, Romania.
  13. Hughes, C., & Graham, A. (2002). Handbook of child psychology and developmemtal science. Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131–142. MacLean, E.W. (1997). Ellis’Handbook of Mental Deficiency, Psychological Theory and Research (Third Edition), edited by William E., MacLean. Jr., Peabody College of Vanderbilt University, Routledge Taylor & Francis Group, New – York and London, pp. 341-362.
  14. Master, R.L. (2010). Ultimate skiing. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  15. Sadeghi, H., Omar-Fauzee, M.S., Jamalis, M., Ab-Latif, R., Cheric, M.C. (2010). The Mental Skills Training of University Soccer Players. Int. Edu. Stu, 3(2), 81-90.
  16. Schack, T., Hackfort, D. (2007). Action-theory approach to applied sport psychology. In: Tenenbaum G & Ecklund RC (Eds.). Handbook of sport psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Son.
  17. Schraw, G.P., & Brooks, D.W. (1999). Improving College Using Interactive, Compensatory Model of Learning.
  18. http://dwb.unl.edu/Chau/CompMod.html (accessed 2/03/2013) Tirri, K., Nokelainen, P. (2011). Measuring Multiple Intelligences and Moral Sensitivities in Education, Oral Development and Citizens Education, Sense Publishers, Rotterdam / Boston / Taipei; Published by Sense Publishers, P.O. Box21858, 3001 AW Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
  19. Unsworth, N., McMillan, B.D., Brewer, G.A., Spillers, G.J. (2012). Everyday Attention Failures: An Individual Differences investigation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. Learning, Memory & Cognition, 38(6), 1765-1772.
  20. Zimmerman, J.B. (2002). Becoming a Self-Regulated Learner: An Overview, Journal Theory Into Practice, 41(2), 64 -70, Copyright c 2002 College of education, The Ohio State University.

Copyright information

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

About this article

Publication Date

04 October 2016

eBook ISBN

978-1-80296-014-3

Publisher

Future Academy

Volume

15

Print ISBN (optional)

-

Edition Number

1st Edition

Pages

1-1115

Subjects

Communication, communication studies, social interaction, moral purpose of education, social purpose of education

Cite this article as:

Grosu, V. T., Grosu, E. F., & Moraru, C. E. (2016). Motor Coordination and Reactivity Influenced by Mental Training in Alpine Skiing. In A. Sandu, T. Ciulei, & A. Frunza (Eds.), Logos Universality Mentality Education Novelty, vol 15. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences (pp. 436-443). Future Academy. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2016.09.56